Skip to main content

Cognitive Revolution, The

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science
  • 58 Accesses

Introduction

The cognitive revolution was an intellectual movement that began in the 1950s and exerted deep influence on psychology, linguistics, computer science, artificial intelligence, neuroscience, and philosophy. It was a reaction against the radical empiricist ways of behaviorism that had dominated the study of human and animal behavior since the early twentieth century.

Background

In the early twentieth century, psychology had wandered a long way from being the “study of mind” that William James had envisioned it to be. Quite the contrary, psychologists had all but given up on issues concerning the mind and the mental, focusing instead on behavior as responses to physical stimuli. The behaviorists argued that mental events, such as beliefs and representations, were not publicly observable. Since my internal beliefs, say “I like red cars,” are not objectively available to others for observation, independently of my introspective recollections, behaviorists argued that such...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aarsleff, H. (1970). The history of linguistics and Professor Chomsky. Language, 570–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. Cognition and the Development of Language, 279(362), 1–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G. (2009). Remarks on the individual basis for linguistic structures. Of Minds and Language: A Dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country, 278–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G., & Poeppel, D. (2010). Analysis by synthesis: A (re-)emerging program of research for language and vision. Biolinguistics, 4(2–3), 174–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G., Fodor, J. A., & Weksel, W. (1965). On the acquisition of syntax: A critique of “contextual generalization.”. Psychological Review, 72(6), 467–482. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G., Lackner, J. R., & Kirk, R. (1969). The underlying structures of sentences are the primary units of immediate speech processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 5(4), 225–234. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C., & Martins, P. T. (2016). Biolinguistics. In Oxford research encyclopedia of linguistics. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosker, H. R. (2016). Neural entrainment as a mechanism behind rate normalization in speech perception. Presented at the Nijmegen Lectures (by David Poeppel).

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnie, A., Medeiros D., & Boeckx, C. (2005). Some consequences of natural law in syntactic structure. Ms. University of Arizona, Harvard University. | Request PDF. (n.d.). Retrieved 26 Nov 2018, from ResearchGate website: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/269699306_Carnie_Andrew_Medeiros_D_and_C_Boeckx_2005_Some_Consequences_of_Natural_Law_in_Syntactic_Structure_Ms_University_of_Arizona_Harvard_University

  • Chomsky, N. (1957/2002). Syntactic structures. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959a). A review of BF Skinner’s verbal behavior. Language, 35(1), 26–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959b). On certain formal properties of grammars. Information and Control, 2(2), 137–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1975). The logical structure of linguistic theory (Vol. 40). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(01), 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2002). On nature and language. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2007). Biolinguistic explorations: Design, development, evolution. International Journal of Philosophical Studies, 15(1), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2009). Cartesian linguistics: A chapter in the history of rationalist thought. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & DiNozzi, R. (1972). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & McGilvray, J. A. (2009). Cartesian linguistics: A chapter in the history of rationalist thought. Cambridge, UK/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Schützenberger, M. P. (1959). The algebraic theory of context-free languages. In Studies in logic and the foundations of mathematics (Vol. 26, pp. 118–161). Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chow, W.-Y., Lewis, S., & Phillips, C. (2014). Immediate sensitivity to structural constraints in pronoun resolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 630.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S., Koring, L., & Thornton, R. (2017). Language acquisition from a biolinguistic perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81, 120–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D., & Poeppel, D. (2015). Towards a computational (ist) neurobiology of language: Correlational, integrated and explanatory neurolinguistics. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(4), 357–366.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2005). The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology and Philosophy, 20(2–3), 193–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought (Vol. 5). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1981). Representations: Philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1998). Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (2001). The mind doesn’t work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J., & Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2011). What Darwin got wrong. London: Profile Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28(1), 3–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R. (1981). Matters of principle: Hierarchies, representations, and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(4), 639–650. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0000073X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R. (2001). Mental representations, psychology of. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (pp. 9691–9695). New York: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01488-1.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. (2007). Commentary on Le Corre & Carey. Cognition, 105(2), 439–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R., & King, A. P. (2009). Memory and the computational brain: Why cognitive science will transform neuroscience. New York: Wiley/Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444310498.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Green, A. A., Kim, J., Ma, D., Silver, P. A., Collins, J. J., & Yin, P. (2017). Complex cellular logic computation using ribocomputing devices. Nature, 548(7665), 117.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, M., & Reiss, C. (2000). “Substance abuse” and “Dysfunctionalism”: Current trends in phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(1), 157–169. https://doi.org/10.1162/002438900554334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, M., & Reiss, C. (2008). The phonological enterprise. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., & Spelke, E. (2004). Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge. The Cognitive Neurosciences, 3, 853–864.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isac, D., & Reiss, C. (2013). I-language: An introduction to linguistics as cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A., & Poeppel, D. (2017). Neuroscience needs behavior: Correcting a reductionist Bias. Neuron, 93(3), 480–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krivochen, D., & Saddy, D. (2018). Towards a classification of Lindenmayer systems. ArXiv Preprint ArXiv, 1809, 10542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed., enl). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H., Chomsky, N., & Marx, O. (1967). Biological foundations of language (Vol. 68). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F. (2003). The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1970). A theory for cerebral neocortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 176(1043), 161–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York, NY: Henry Holt and co. Inc. 2(4.2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D., & Thach, W. T. (1991). A theory of cerebellar cortex. In From the Retina to the Neocortex (pp. 11–50). Boston: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D., Willshaw, D., & McNaughton, B. (1991). Simple memory: A theory for archicortex. In From the Retina to the Neocortex (pp. 59–128). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134(3489), 1501–1506.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medeiros, D. P., Piattelli-Palmarini, M., & Bever, T. G. (2016). Many important language universals are not reducible to processing or cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000722.

  • Miller, G. A. (1951). Language and communication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (1956a). Human memory and the storage of information. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 129–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956b). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 141–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., & Chomsky, N. (1963). Finitary models of language users. In D. Luce (ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 2–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1961). Steps toward artificial intelligence. Proceedings of the IRE, 49(1), 8–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M., & Papert, S. A. (2017). Perceptrons: An introduction to computational geometry. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1956). The logic theory machine – A complex information processing system. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 61–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2013). Biolinguistics yesterday, today, and tomorrow. The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics, 12–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M., & Uriagereka, J. (2004). The immune syntax: The evolution of the language virus. In Variation and Universals in Biolinguistics. Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M., & Uriagereka, J. (2008). Still a bridge too far? Biolinguistic questions for grounding language on brains. Physics of Life Reviews, 5(4), 207–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M., & Vitiello, G. (2017). Quantum field theory and the linguistic minimalist program: A remarkable isomorphism. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 880, 012016. https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-6596/880/1/012016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. 1997. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 95(2), 201–236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poeppel, D., & Embick, D. (2017). Defining the relation between linguistics and neuroscience. In Twenty-first century psycholinguistics (pp. 103–118). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poeppel, D., Emmorey, K., Hickok, G., & Pylkkänen, L. (2012). Towards a new neurobiology of language. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(41), 14125–14131.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, C. (2003). Quantification in structural descriptions: Attested and unattested patterns. Linguistic Review, 20(2/4), 305–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, C. (2007). Modularity in the “sound” domain: Implications for the purview of universal grammar. The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199247455.013.0003.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti, M., Alkadhi, H., Moro, A., Perani, D., Kollias, S., & Weniger, D. (2002). Neural correlates for the Acquisition of Natural Language Syntax. NeuroImage, 17(2), 700–709. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti, M., Rotondi, I., Perani, D., Scotti, G., Fazio, F., Cappa, S. F., & Moro, A. (2009). Syntax without language: Neurobiological evidence for cross-domain syntactic computations. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(7), 825–838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.11.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volenec, V., & Reiss, C. (2017). Cognitive Phonetics: The Transduction of Distinctive Features at the Phonology-Phonetics Interface. Biolinguistics, 11, 251–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, A. N., & Russell, B. (1912). Principia mathematica (Vol. 2). University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (2000). Sociobiology. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. M., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Neural responses to non-native phonemes varying in producibility: Evidence for the sensorimotor nature of speech perception. NeuroImage, 33(1), 316–325.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xiang, M., Dillon, B., & Phillips, C. (2009). Illusory licensing effects across dependency types: ERP evidence. Brain and Language, 108(1), 40–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sayantan Mandal .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Mandal, S. (2020). Cognitive Revolution, The. In: Shackelford, T., Weekes-Shackelford, V. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1309-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1309-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics