Skip to main content

From Causes to Outcomes: Determining Prevention Can Work

  • Chapter
Advances in Child Abuse Prevention Knowledge

Part of the book series: Child Maltreatment ((MALT,volume 5))

  • 1578 Accesses

Abstract

To be successful in practice with families and to gain support for further dissemination, maltreatment prevention interventions must accomplish two goals of program design and evaluation: (1) develop and implement a logic model informed by theory that targets known causes of child maltreatment, and (2) demonstrate evidence of effectiveness with rigorous methodological designs that isolate program effects. The current chapter focuses on the role of two types of causal inquiry for research in child maltreatment prevention. The chapter begins with a discussion of how theories on maltreatment etiology inform logic models for existing prevention programs (“causes of known effects”). We then move to a summary of exciting statistical methods that bring us closer to inferring causality in observational studies (“effects of known causes”). The chapter ends with reflections on methodological advances for the future of maltreatment prevention, with a discussion on how to continue to move the field forward.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The reader is directed to Pearl (1988, 2009) for a more formal and complete treatment of DAGs and d-separation.

  2. 2.

    We direct the reader to Heckman (1997) for a discussion of the use of instrumental variables in program evaluation and to Hogan and Lancaster (2004) for application in longitudinal studies of public health outcomes.

  3. 3.

    As with all DAGs, the depiction represents relationships between variables. These variables can take on many potential values. In the case of home visiting, a child can be assigned to the home visiting program (i.e., Home Visiting = 1) or not (i.e., Home Visiting = 0). Both conditions are implicit in the single vertex.

  4. 4.

    Propensity score matching is just one type of propensity score analysis. Guo and Fraser’s (2010) text provides a useful review of the need for propensity score methods in observational studies and provides examples of software analysis code and output.

  5. 5.

    For additional information on the design and implementation of an evaluation system for program components, see McCabe et al. 2012.

  6. 6.

    For more information on missing data see Enders (2010). For the strategies mentioned, data that are missing have to assume missing at random (MAR) or missing completely at random (MCAR) types, which means that there is little to no correlation between the variable that caused the missingness and the variable containing the missingness. By incorporating planned missing data designs, MAR and MCAR assumptions can be under the researchers’ control (Graham et al. 2006).

  7. 7.

    For further information on two-method design issues such as power, sample size considerations, and effect sizes, please see Graham et al. (2006) and Little and Rhemtulla (2013).

  8. 8.

    http://obssr.od.nih.gov/scientific_areas/translation/index.aspx?p=104

References

  • Barth, R., Landsverk, J., Chamberlain, P., Reid, J., Rolls, J., Hurlburt, M., et al. (2005). Parent-training programs in child welfare services: Planning for a more evidence-based approach to serving biological parents. Research on Social Work Practice, 15(5), 353–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration. American Psychologist, 35(4), 320–335. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.35.4.320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Pearl, J. (2013). Eight myths about causality and structural equation models. In S. L. Morgan (Ed.), Handbook of causal analysis for social research (pp. 301–328). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32(7), 513–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Theoretical models of human development 6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 793–828). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruskas, D. (2008). Children in foster care: A vulnerable population at risk. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 21(2), 70–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, V., & Myers, M. (2007). Exploring the risks of substantiated physical neglect related to poverty and parental characteristics: A national sample. Children and Youth Services Review, 29, 110–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, P., Roberts, R., Jones, H., Marsenich, L., Sosna, T., & Price, J. (2012). Three collaborative models for scaling up evidence-based practices. Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 39, 278–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children’s development. Psychiatry, 56, 96–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Rizley, R. (1981). Developmental perspectives on the etiology, intergenerational transmission, and sequelae of child maltreatment. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1981(11), 31–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coca-Perraillon, M. (2007). Local and global optimal propensity score matching. SAS Global Forum. Paper 185–2007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coie, J. D., Watt, N. F., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., Asarnow, J. R., Markman, H. J., et al. (1993). The science of prevention: A conceptual framework and some directions for a national research program. American Psychologist, 48(10), 1013–1022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, L., & Algina, J. (1986). Introduction to classical and modern test theory. Philadelphia, PA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Agostino, R. (1998). Tutorial in biostatistics: Propensity score methods for bias reduction in the comparison of a treatment to a non-randomized control group. Statistics in Medicine, 17, 2265–2281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daro, D. (1988). Confronting child abuse: Research for effective program design. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, J. J., Jr. (2011). Causal effects of foster care: An instrumental-variables approach. Children and Youth Services Review, 35(7), 1143–1151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lindhiem, O., Gordon, M. K., Manni, M., Sepulveda, S., et al. (2006). Developing evidence-based interventions for foster children: An example of a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. Journal of Social Issues, 62(4), 767–785.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dozier, M., Lindhiem, O., Lewis, E., Bick, J., Bernard, K., & Peloso, E. (2009). Effects of a foster parent training program on young children’s attachment behaviors: Preliminary evidence from a randomized clinical trial. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 26, 321–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garbarino, J. (1977). The human ecology of child maltreatment: A conceptual model for research. Journal of Marriage and Family, 39(4), 721–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gil, D. (1970). Violence against children: Physical child abuse in the United States. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J. W., Taylor, B. J., Cumsille, P. E., & Olchowski, A. E. (2006). Planned missing data designs in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 11, 323–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeley, C. S. (2009). The future of child maltreatment prevention. Pediatrics, 123(3), 904–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo, S., & Fraser, M. W. (2010). Propensity score analysis: Statistical methods and applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guo, S., Barth, R., & Gibbons, C. (2006). Propensity score matching strategies for evaluating substance abuse services for child welfare clients. Children and Youth Services Review, 28(4), 357–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, W. R. (2003). Public health and child maltreatment prevention: The role of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Child Maltreatment, 8(2), 81–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. (1997). Instrumental variables: A study of implicit behavioral assumptions used in making program evaluations. Journal of Human Resources, 32(3), 441–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herschell, A. D., Calzada, E. J., Eyberg, S. M., & McNeil, C. B. (2002). Parent–child interaction therapy: New directions in research. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 9, 9–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, J. W., & Lancaster, T. (2004). Instrumental variables and inverse probability weighting for causal inference from longitudinal observational studies. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 13, 17–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogue, C. M., Porprasertmanit, S., Fry, M. D., Rhemtulla, M., & Little, T. (2013). Planned missing data designs for spline growth models in salivary cortisol research. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 17, 310–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P. W. (1986). Statistics and causal inference. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 81(396), 945–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1748). Philosophical Essays concerning Human Understanding. London: A. Millar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Institute of Medicine & National Research Council. (2012). Child maltreatment research, policy, and practice for the next decade: Workshop summary. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jo, B. (2002). Statistical power in randomized intervention studies with noncompliance. Psychological Methods, 7, 178–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellam, S. G., & Langevin, D. J. (2003). A framework for understanding “evidence” in prevention research and programs. Prevention Science, 4(3), 137–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, C. H., Silverman, F. N., Steele, B. F., Droegemueller, W., & Silver, H. K. (1962). The battered-child syndrome. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 181(1), 17–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., & Rhemtulla, M. (2013). Planned missing data designs for developmental researchers. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 199–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). An ecological-transactional analysis of children and contexts: The longitudinal interplay among child maltreatment, community violence, and children’s symptomatology. Development and Psychopathology, 10(2), 235–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacMillan, H., Wathen, C., Barlow, J., Fergusson, D., Leventhal, J., & Taussig, H. (2009). Interventions to prevent child maltreatment and associated impairment. Lancet, 373(9659), 250–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, B. K., Potash, D., Omohundro, E., & Taylor, C. R. (2012). Design and implementation of an integrated, continuous evaluation, and quality improvement system for a state-based home-visiting program. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 16, 1385–1400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, J. A., & Louis, T. (2010, January). Regression adjustment and stratification by propensity score in treatment effect estimation (Working Paper 203). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, Department of Biostatistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (1993). Understanding child abuse and neglect. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neyman, J. (1990). On the application of probability theory to agricultural experiments: Essay on principles. Section 9 (Splawa-Neyman, J., Dabrowska, D. M., & Speed, T. P., Trans.). Statistical Science, 5, 465–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olds, D. (2006). The nurse-family partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27(1), 5–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olds, D., Kitzman, H., Cole, R., & Robinson, J. (1997). Theoretical foundations of a program of home visitation for pregnant women and parents of young children. Journal of Community Psychology, 25(1), 9–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parrish, J. W., Young, M. B., Perham-Hester, K. A., & Gessner, B. D. (2011). Identifying risk factors for child maltreatment in Alaska: A population-based approach. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 40(6), 666–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J. (1988). Probabilistic reasoning in intelligent systems. San Mateo: Morgan Kaufman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J. (2009). Causality: Models, reasoning and inference. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J., & Verma, T. S. (1995). A theory of inferred causation. Studies in Logic and the Foundations of Mathematics, 134, 789–811.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, R., Sanders, M., Shapiro, C., Whitaker, D., & Lutzker, J. (2009). Population-based prevention of child maltreatment: The U.S. triple P system population trial. Prevention Science, 10(1), 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam-Hornstein, E., & Needell, B. (2011). Predictors of child protective service contact between birth and age five: An examination of California’s 2002 birth cohort. Children and Youth Services Review, 33(8), 1337–1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. R. (1983). The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika, 70(1), 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (1974). Estimating causal effects of treatments in randomized and nonrandomized studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(5), 688–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (2010). Propensity score methods. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 149(1), 7–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, D. P. (2008). State actors beating children: A call for judicial relief. University of California Davis Law Review, 42, 1165–1230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidebotham, P. (2001). An ecological approach to child abuse: A creative use of scientific models in research and practice. Child Abuse Review, 10(2), 97–112. doi:10.1002/car.643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slack, K. S., Maguire-Jack, K., & Gjertson, L. M. (Eds.) (2009). Child maltreatment prevention: Toward an evidence-based approach. Madison: Institute for Research on Poverty. http://www.irp.wisc.edu/research/WisconsinPoverty/pdfs/ChildMaltreatment-Final.pdf

  • Standards of Evidence Committee. (2004). Standards of evidence: Criteria for efficacy, effectiveness, and dissemination. Fairfax: Society for Prevention Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele, B. F., & Pollack, G. (1974). A psychiatric study of parents who abuse their children and infants. In C. H. Kempe (Ed.), The battered child (pp. 89–133). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strathearn, L., Gray, P., O’Callaghan, M., & Wood, D. (2001). Childhood neglect and cognitive development in extremely low birth weight infants: A prospective study. Pediatrics, 108, 142–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, E. A., Cole, S. R., Bradshaw, C. P., & Leaf, P. J. (2011). The use of propensity scores to assess the generalizability of results from randomized trials. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 174, 369–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thoemmes, F. J., & Kim, E. S. (2011). A systematic review of propensity score methods in the social sciences. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46(1), 90–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, O. C., Jackson, J. W., & Karlson, H. C. (1991). Theories of child abuse and neglect. Differential perspectives, summaries, and evaluations. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widom, C. (1989). The cycle of violence. Science, 244(4901), 160–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widom, C. (2000). Understanding the consequences of childhood victimization. In R. Reece (Ed.), Treatment of child abuse: Common ground for mental health, medical, and legal practitioners (pp. 339–361). Baltimore/London: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widom, C., & Brzustowicz, L. (2006). MAOA and the “cycle of violence”: Childhood abuse and neglect, MAOA genotype, and risk for violent and antisocial behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 684–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. S. (1921). Correlation and causation. Journal of Agricultural Research, 20, 557–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, S. S., Ma, C. X., Carter, R. L., Ariet, M., Feaver, E. A., Resnick, M. B., & Roth, J. (2004). Risk factors for infant maltreatment: A population-based study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28(12), 1253–1264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelkowitz, P., Bardin, C., & Papageorgiou, A. (2007). Anxiety affects the relationship between parents and their very low birth weight infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28(3), 296–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zielinski, D., & Bradshaw, C. (2006). Ecological influences on the sequalae of child maltreatment: A review of the literature. Child Maltreatment, 11(1), 49–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zolotor, A. J., & Puzia, M. E. (2010). Bans against corporal punishment: A systematic review of the laws, changes in attitudes and behaviours. Child Abuse Review, 19(4), 229–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Lanier .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Reflection: Nature of Evidence and How We Use It

Reflection: Nature of Evidence and How We Use It

Enormous changes have occurred in the last 50 years in how we define evidence and apply it to public policy. My own direct experience with how these changes have played out began when I joined the War on Poverty at its inception. Dozens of initiatives were inaugurated based on high hopes, innovative ideas, and evidence of varying strengths. Efforts to be more systematic focused on recruiting several of Robert McNamara’s “whiz kids” from the Pentagon to calculate cost-effectiveness ratios. (Their only unambiguous contribution in the early stages was finding that the cost-effectiveness of a proposed family planning program came in at 17 to 1, allowing the Office of Economic Opportunity to establish the first federal line item funding for contraceptive services. Reflecting the climate of the time, these would be provided only to married women over 21!)

The importance of establishing clear, quantifiable evidence of what works has grown throughout my career. But the current narrow view that randomized control trials and similar experiments are the only credible information to inform decisions about what is worth funding has overshot the mark and undermined many efforts to improve outcomes. You cannot unravel the “why” and “how” if all your knowledge comes from studies that hold an intervention constant and isolated, as though it were operating in a laboratory setting. This emphasis on proving that a defined model “works” has obscured how different contexts—the institutional system, the funding streams, and the participants and their community—influence and shape an intervention’s potential to be effective. Insisting that “proven” models be implemented with fidelity discourages local agencies and funders from reaching new populations, addressing emerging issues, and acting on lessons learned.

To make significant progress, we must determine the essential elements of our efforts and then carefully document, on a day to day basis, how our actions impact our ability to achieve outcomes. We need to “steer as we go” and accept the fact that there is no straight line between cause and effect—we need to tolerate some messiness. Failure to be comfortable with “messy” results pushes us away from the type of complex interventions that are needed to successfully reduce child maltreatment and confront other complex problems, like race-, class- and income-based gaps in well-being and achievement.

The key to improvement is not solely a function of creating better program models. Learning about how and why and how well an intervention works, and explicitly taking account of the importance of context, is a different type of learning. This approach takes us beyond yes/no judgments of a defined model and allows us to rank programs not in terms of the elegance with which they have been evaluated, but rather by our understanding of the strategy’s potential to improve defined outcomes. It would allow us to make judgments beyond individual programs to identify strategies, including the interactions among multiple programs and reforms of systems and policies, that could achieve transformative outcomes.

We have come a long way since the War on Poverty. We no longer rely on justifications or anecdotes that the program worked for someone, somewhere as a basis for allocating resources. Gathering, analyzing, and applying a broader range of evidence is particularly important when it comes to initiatives aimed at prevention, where the need to understand context is critical. Building a deeper and wider knowledge base will require the ability to understand the critical elements of diverse interventions that focus on similar outcomes. At present, there are not many vehicles that allow program developers or funders to do that type of cross-strategy learning. In order to develop a broader, deeper understanding of how we might improve outcomes, we need learning communities and learning networks that can build on one another’s experience and learn together from research to build a knowledge base and evidence pool sufficiently sturdy to lead to meaningful improvements at scale in the outcomes we most care about.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lanier, P., Maguire-Jack, K., Mienko, J., Panlilio, C. (2015). From Causes to Outcomes: Determining Prevention Can Work. In: Daro, D., Cohn Donnelly, A., Huang, L., Powell, B. (eds) Advances in Child Abuse Prevention Knowledge. Child Maltreatment, vol 5. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16327-7_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics