Skip to main content

Nonlinear Analysis of the Effect of Working Memory Capacity on Student Performance in Problem Solving

  • Chapter
Science Education Research in the Knowledge-Based Society

Abstract

We describe a nonlinear approach for studying the effect of working-memory capacity on science problem solving. The tools used are those of complexity theory and are used to test and model the phenomenon of the working-memory overload. The nonlinear method correlates the subjects’ rank-order achievement scores in problem solving with the psychometric variable. From the achievement scores, rank-order sequences of the subjects, according to their scores, were generated, and in the place of each subject, his/her score was then replaced by the value of the cognitive variable. Then the sequence was mapped onto a one-dimensional random-walk model (working-memory random walk) and treated as dynamical flow. For these sequences, the nonlinear correlation exponents (Hurst exponents) were calculated. The null hypothesis was tested with surrogate data. The method is demonstrated with data from achievement scores in chemical equilibrium problems. Problems of various logical structures and mental demands were used. In problems with simpler logical structure and with low mental demand, the Hurst exponent was close to the surrogate value (demonstrating randomness), while sudden increases appeared for more complicated problems (indicating scale invariant long-range correlations). The method provides meaningful results and adds to the understanding of information processing within the frame of science education.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Johnstone, A. H., (1984). New stars for the teacher to steer by? Journal of Chemical Education, 61, 847–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H. & El-Banna, H., (1986). Capacities, demands and processes — A predictive model for science education. Education in Chemistry, 23, 80–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H., Hogg, W. R., & Ziane, M., (1993). A working memory model applied to physics problem solving. Internationaljournal of Science Education, 15, 663–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H. & Kellet, N. C, (1980). Learning difficulties in school science — Towards a working hypothesis. European Journal of Science Education, 2, 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H. & Selepeg, D., (2000). A Language Problem Revisited. Chemistry Education: Research And Practice In Europe (vol. 2, pp. 19–20). (CERAPIE). [http://www.uoi.gr/conf_sem/cerapie]

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantz, H. & Schreiber, T. N., (1999). Nonlinear time series analysis. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. & Loggie, R. H., (1993). Working memory, mental capacity, and science education: Towards an understanding of the ‘working memory overload hypothesis’. Oxford Review of Education, 19, 511–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., (1969). The encoding and decoding of symbols by children: A new experimental paradigm and neo-Piagetian model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 328–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scandarmalia, M. (1977). Information processing capacity and the problem of horizontal decalage: A demonstration using combinatorial reasoning task. Child Psychology, 48, 301–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprott, J. C. & Rowlands, G., (1995). Chaos data analyzer. Physical Academy Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stamovlasis, D. & Tsaparlis G., (1999). An application of complexity theory to an information model in science education. Paper presented at the 9th International Conference for Chaos Theory in Psychology and Life Sciences. Berkeley University, CA, USA: July 23–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stamovlasis, D. & Tsaparlis G., (2000). Nonlinear analysis of the effect of working-memory capacity on organic-synthesis problem solving. Chemistry Education: Research and Practice in Europe (CERAPIE), 1, 375–380 [http://www.uoi.gr/conf_sem/cerapie]

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stamovlasis, D. & Tsaparlis G., (2001a). Application of complexity theory to an information-processing model in science education. Nonlinear Dynamics in Psychology and Life Sciences, 5, 267–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stamovlasis D. & Tsaparlis G. (2001b). Nonlinear analysis of the effect of working memory capacity on student performance in problem solving: the case of chemical equilibrium problem solving. Proceedings of the third International conference of the European Science Education Research Association (ESERA), August 21–27, 2001, Thessaloniki, Macedonia, Greece.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stamovlasis, D. & Tsaparlis G., (2002). A complexity theory model in science education problem solving: Random walks for working memory and mental capacity. Nonlinear Dynamics in Psychology and Life Sciences. Accepted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsaparlis, G., (1998). Dimensional analysis and predictive models in problem solving. International Journal of Science Education, 20, 335–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsaparlis, G., & Angelopoulos, V., (2000). A model of problem solving: Its operation, validity, and usefulness in the case of organic-synthesis problems. Science Education, 84, 131–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsaparlis G. & Stamovlasis D., (1999). A working-memory random walk for analyzing problem-solving data: The case of organic-synthesis problems. Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference of the European Science Education Research Association (pp. 226–228), (ESERA). Kiel, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsaparlis, G., Kousathana, M., & Niaz, M., (1998). Molecular-equilibrium problems: Manipulation of logical structure and of M-demand, and their effect on student performance. Science Education, 82, 437–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D., (1955). Wechsler adult intelligence scale manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Stamovlasis, D., Tsaparlis, G. (2003). Nonlinear Analysis of the Effect of Working Memory Capacity on Student Performance in Problem Solving. In: Psillos, D., Kariotoglou, P., Tselfes, V., Hatzikraniotis, E., Fassoulopoulos, G., Kallery, M. (eds) Science Education Research in the Knowledge-Based Society. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0165-5_20

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0165-5_20

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-6337-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-0165-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics