Introduction

Holistic processing (HP) is a concept originally derived from Gestalt psychology that postulates that the perception of an object as a whole is qualitatively different from the sum of its parts. HP has been commonly observed in face perception – one example is the composite face illusion, in which two identical top half-faces are judged to be different when they are paired with different bottom halves. This illusion reflects obligatorily attention to all facial features as a whole (Richler, Wong, & Gauthier, 2011). Beyond face perception, some have posited that expert subordinate-level object recognition requires HP (Bukach, Gauthier, & Tarr, 2006).

Chinese characters share many visual properties with faces. They have a homogenous, square configuration – with each character a grapheme mapping onto a morpheme. Strokes are the basic units of the orthography, which combine to form more than 200 basic stroke patterns (Hsiao & Shillcock, 2006), which in turn form the characters. A person can recognize different faces regardless of their facial expressions, similar to a literate needing to recognize over 3,000 characters regardless of fonts. Hence, theoretically, Chinese characters should induce similar perceptual expertise effects as faces do (Hsiao & Cottrell, 2009).

However, in contrast to the literature suggesting HP as a perceptual expertise marker, Hsiao and Cottrell (2009) found that expert Chinese readers had a reduced HP effect compared with novices. Tso, Au, and Hsiao (2014) further showed that this reduced HP effect in expert readers may be explained by writing performances: Compared with novices, proficient readers with limited writing performances (Limited-writers) showed increased HP, while expert readers with typical writing abilities (Writers) showed reduced HP. These findings hint at a modulating role of writing abilities in HP. Tso et al. (2014) also found that, while Limited-writers had comparable character naming accuracy to Writers, they had significantly longer response times, suggesting less efficient character processing. Thus, expert Chinese character recognition involves the ability to selectively attend to and efficiently identify character components, as demonstrated in the reduced HP effect.

Left-side bias (LSB) is another perceptual phenomenon commonly reported in face perception: A chimeric face created by fusing two left half-faces together is often judged more similar to the original face than one created by fusing two right half-faces (from the viewer’s perspective; Brady, Campbell, & Flaherty, 2005; Fig. 1). The LSB effect has also been demonstrated in expert Chinese character recognition and is suggested to be associated with right-hemisphere (RH) involvement in face and Chinese character recognition (Brady et al., 2005; Hsiao, Shillcock, & Lee, 2007).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Examples of chimeric face stimuli (adopted from Hsiao & Cottrell, 2009). The left chimeric face was formed by combining two left halves of an original face (middle), while the right chimeric face was formed by the two right halves

Though LSB, or RH lateralization, has been thought to correlate with increase in HP in visual object recognition (Gauthier & Tarr, 2002), expert Chinese character recognition has been reported to involve increased LSB and decreased HP, presenting a counter example (Hsiao & Cottrell, 2009). Tso et al. (2014) added that LSB is a consistent expertise marker for Chinese characters unaffected by readers’ writing experiences, since Writers and Limited-writers showed a similar level of LSB. Through both computational modeling and human subject experiments with face stimuli, Hsiao and Galmar (2016) showed that the relationship between RH lateralization and HP in visual object recognition depends on whether the recognition task relies on featural information or configural information (i.e., exact distances, or second-order relations among features; see Maurer, Grand, & Mondloch, 2002): a positive correlation was observed when the task relies purely on configural information, whereas a negative correlation was found when the task relies purely on featural information. In Chinese character recognition, featural information (e.g., individual strokes) is more critical to character identification than configural information (e.g., exact distances between strokes). Accordingly, HP and LSB in Chinese character-recognition expertise would not go together, consistent with the finding of decreased HP and increased LSB reported in Hsiao and Cottrell (2009). Chung, Leung, Wong, and Hsiao (2018a) further showed that when Chinese characters were presented to the left visual field/RH, Chinese novice readers showed HP effects whereas expert readers did not. This suggests that the RH may flexibly switch between holistic and part-based representations, consistent with a recent fMRI study (Harris & Aguirre, 2010). Chung et al. (2018) argued that the advantage of the RH in low spatial frequency processing is relative to the task-relevant frequency range (Ivry & Robertson, 1998), which depends on whether attention is allocated to the whole stimulus or to the parts. Thus, the RH can be involved in forming a low spatial frequency biased representation for either the whole or a part of the stimulus based on the task demand.

The understanding of the relationship between HP and RH lateralization gives us important insights into the mechanism underlying deficits in visual object recognition. For example, patients with prosopagnosia (clinical difficulties in face recognition) were reported to have reduced HP (Avidan, Tanzer, and Behrmann, 2011) and LSB in face perception (Malaspina, Albonic, & Daini, 2016). These results suggest that their deficits may be related to difficulties in forming appropriate configural representations in the RH. In contrast, HP and LSB effects in Chinese readers with reading difficulty (dyslexia) remain unclear. Difficulties in Chinese character recognition may be related to failure to de-emphasize configural information unimportant for character identification; this may lead to LSB with increased HP as compared with typically developing readers, such as the Limited-writers reported in Tso et al. (2014). Alternatively, if Chinese dyslexics show increased HP and reduced LSB as compared with controls, it would suggest that their deficits may be related to inability to selectively attend to character parts to form appropriate part representations in the RH rather than inability to deemphasize configural information.

While dyslexia in alphabetic languages is generally associated with core deficits in phonological skills and corresponding neurological deficits, dyslexia in Chinese is more associated with abnormalities in orthographic or visuospatial processing (Siok et al., 2004). In particular, character writing and copying abilities play an important role in Chinese literacy development (Tan et al., 2005), and children with Chinese reading difficulties often have a marked discrepancy between reading and writing abilities (Chung & Ho, 2010). As writing abilities seem to be particularly relevant to the reduced HP effect in expert Chinese readers (Tso et al., 2014), people with dyslexia may fail to employ analytic (reduced holistic) processing. In addition, Siok et al. (2004) showed reduced activities in the RH during Chinese character processing in dyslexics when compared with controls. Together these findings suggest that Chinese readers with dyslexia may show increased HP and reduced LSB in character perception as compared with controls, which would suggest their inability to selectively attend to character parts to form appropriate part representations in the RH. Here we tested this hypothesis through recruiting adolescents in Hong Kong who were native Chinese speakers and readers with and without dyslexia and examining their difference in HP and LSB in Chinese character perception.

Methods

Participants

Twenty typically developing students and 20 students diagnosed with dyslexia were recruited from a secondary school in Hong Kong, with four females and 16 males in each group. This sample size was determined by taking into consideration the sample size in the Tso et al. (2014) study, in which a large effect could be detected with a sample size of 40 participants. Dyslexia in Hong Kong is typically diagnosed by a clinical or educational psychologist using standardized test batteries as having normal IQs and performing at least one standard deviation below their same-age peers in the literacy domain and at least one literacy-relative cognitive subtest using the Hong Kong Test of Specific Learning Difficulties in Reading and Writing for Primary School StudentsThird Edition [HKT-P(III)] (Ho et al., 2016). Participants with dyslexia had been identified and diagnosed by a qualified psychologist prior to joining this study, and all reported no other co-morbid learning disabilities. All of them reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The two groups were matched in age (M = 14.6 years, S.E. = .26), F(1, 39) = 1, p = .753.

Materials and procedures

Chinese literacy

Dictation and Chinese word reading performance in both timed and untimed contexts were measured as a reference for the participants’ literacy performances. The stimuli were adopted from HKT-P(III). Previous pilot studies have shown that the HKT-P(III) could even differentiate college students with dyslexia from those without. As our purpose was not to yield diagnostic results, the use of the stimuli from HKT-P(III) was for research purposes only to provide a comparison of literacy performances between students with dyslexia and those without.

The subtests administered were: (1) the Untimed Chinese word reading task, which assessed word reading accuracy; (2) the Chinese one-minute word reading task, which assessed word reading fluency; and (3) the Chinese dictation task, which assessed word-writing ability.

Orthographic awareness

The lexical decision task was adopted from Ho et al. (2016) to assess students’ performance in discriminating orthographically legal characters from illegal ones. Their response time to finish the task was measured.

Rapid automatized naming

Because general symbol-processing speed is related to reading ability in children (Wolf & Bowers, 1999), we used the Rapid Digit Naming task from HKT-P(III) to measure rapid automatized naming (RAN) of symbol-processing speed. This test contains eight rows of five digits (2, 4, 6, 7, 9) arranged in a random sequence in each row. Participants were instructed to read the digits aloud as quickly and accurately as they could. Each participant attempted the task twice and the score was calculated by the average time to complete.

Nonverbal intelligence

To control for the effect of IQ on reading, nonverbal intelligence was assessed using the nine-item subset of Raven’s standard progressing matrices (Bilker et al., 2012).

Holistic processing

Following Hsiao and Cottrell (2009) and Tso et al. (2014), the complete composite paradigm was used to assess HP effects. 160 pairs of medium- to high-frequency Chinese characters in Ming font were used – half of the pairs in a top-bottom configuration and the other half in a left-right configuration (Tso et al., 2014;Footnote 1 Fig. 2). Forty pairs were presented in each of the four conditions in Fig. 3a. In the congruent trials, the attended halves and the irrelevant halves led to the same response. In the incongruent trials, the attended halves and the irrelevant halves led to different responses.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Examples of Chinese characters with a left-right configuration (left) and a top-bottom configuration (right)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Illustration of stimulus pairs in the complete composite paradigm and trial sequences. In (a), it shows the four conditions used in the paradigm; the attended components are shaded in grey. In (b), a 1,000-ms central fixation cross preceded each trial, followed by a cue either below or above the cross, or to the left or right of the cross, to indicate which halves (top or bottom/left or right) of the characters participants should attend to in the subsequent display. (c) A character in aligned (left) and misaligned conditions (right)

The same procedure as that in Tso et al. (2014) was used (Fig. 3b). Participants’ performance in each condition was measured by discrimination sensitivity A' as:

$$ A\hbox{'}=0.5+\left[\mathit{\operatorname{sign}}\left(H-F\right)\frac{{\left(H-F\right)}^2+\left|H-F\right|}{4\max \left(H,F\right)-4 HF}\right] $$

where H and F are the hit and false-alarm rate, respective. A' was used due to its bias-free nonparametric property (Hsiao & Cottrell, 2009). HP was measured by A' or response time difference between the congruent and incongruent trials – the larger the discrepancy, the larger the HP effect. In addition, a misaligned condition was included such that if the HP effect was due to interference from the irrelevant halves, misalignment would reduce this effect (Fig. 3b).

Left-side bias

The procedure from Tso et al. (2014) was adopted (Fig. 4). Eighty high-frequency mirror-symmetric Chinese characters were used. Each character was presented once in Ming font. Half of the trials displayed the original characters, whereas the other half displayed characters in mirror image; this was counter-balanced across participants. For each stimulus, two left halves constructed the left chimeric character while two right halves formed the right chimeric character. LSB was measured as the percentage of trials in which participants selected the left chimeric character as more similar to the original character.

Fig. 4
figure 4

(a) Examples of the stimuli, and (b) the test sequence in the LSB experiment (note that the chimeric characters are still legal Chinese characters). Each character spanned a visual angle of about 6.7° with a 55-cm viewing distance. After a 500-ms central fixation cross in each trial, the original character was displayed either on the left or right side of the screen randomly, at about 7.2° of visual angle away from the center. Simultaneously, the left and right chimeric characters were presented with one above and one below an arrow at the screen center. The arrow pointed to the original character. Each chimeric character was about 3°of visual angle away from the center. All stimuli were displayed until the participant responded, by pressing corresponding buttons on a response box, to judge which of the two chimeric characters looked more similar to the original one

Results

Literacy abilities and non-verbal intelligence

Separate independent sample t-tests were carried out to examine the effect of group (Dyslexics vs. Control) on each literacy test (Tables 1 and 2). Participants with dyslexia and matched controls had similar performances in Chinese word reading and 1-min word reading. However, the controls had marginally better performance in Chinese word dictation, suggesting that the students with dyslexia had persistent difficulties in writing Chinese characters even in high-school grades. The participants with dyslexia were also slower in RAN than the controls, suggesting that they had persistent deficits in automatized naming of orthographic symbols. The controls also had significantly faster response times in lexical decision than the dyslexics, suggesting the dyslexics were slower at discriminating orthographically legal characters from orthographically illegal ones.Footnote 2

Table 1 Summary of the scores of Chinese word-reading, Chinese 1-min word reading, Chinese dictation, and non-verbal IQ (nine-item Raven's) in high-school students with and without dyslexia
Table 2 Results of independent-sample t-tests (dyslexics vs. Control) for Chinese word-reading, Chinese 1-min word reading, Chinese dictation, and non-verbal IQ (nine-item Raven's) in high-school students with and without dyslexia

Holistic processing

We conducted a 2 (congruency: congruent vs. incongruent) × 2 (group: dyslexics vs. control) repeated-measures ANOVA on the data in the aligned condition to examine HP effects. In A', there was a main effect of congruency, F(1, 38) = 27.35, p = .000006, ηp2 = .419, but no interaction between congruency and group, F(1, 38) = 1.354, p = .252, or main effect of group, F(1, 38) = 1.342, p = .254. In response time, we found a significant interaction between congruency and group, F(1, 38) = 5.854, p = .02, ηp2 = .133, and a main effect of group, F(1, 38) = 5.306, p = .027, ηp2 = .123, but no main effect of congruency, F(1, 38) = 2.254, p = .150. Post hoc repeated-measures ANOVA showed that students with dyslexia responded more slowly in incongruent than in congruent trials, F(19) = 34.3, p < .000012, ηp2 = .644, whereas response times in congruent and incongruent trials were similar in typically developing students, F(19) = 0.254, p = .620 (Fig. 5). To examine whether the congruency effect observed in the students with dyslexia was indeed due to interference from the irrelevant halves, following Hsiao and Cottrell (2009), we conducted a 2 (congruent vs. incongruent) × 2 (aligned vs. misaligned) repeated-measures ANOVA in the two participant groups separately. The results showed that misalignment significantly reduced the congruency effect demonstrated in response time in the students with dyslexia: a significant interaction was found between congruency and misalignment, F(1, 19) = 5.662, p = .029, ηp2 = .239. There was no misalignment effect in typically developing students. These results suggest that participants with dyslexia perceived Chinese characters more holistically than the controls.Footnote 3

Fig. 5
figure 5

A' and response time (RT) in the complete composite paradigm are shown in (a, b, c, and d). The holistic processing (congruency) effect in character perception was significant for dyslexics (c), but not the typically developing readers (d) as shown in the RT. A' did not reveal significant group differences as shown in (a) and (b). Misalignments significantly reduced the effect in the dyslexics as shown in RT. The intercepting line in the box denotes the group median, x represents the mean, while the box denotes data points from the first to third quartiles. The range is represented by the whiskers

Left-side bias

The results showed that the typical readers had a stronger LSB than the students with dyslexia, F (1,38) = 6.439, p = .015, ηp2 = .145 (Fig. 6). Thus, although those with dyslexia were more holistic than the typical readers in Chinese character recognition, they revealed a weaker LSB.Footnote 4

Fig. 6
figure 6

Left-side bias effect in Chinese character perception was larger in typically developing readers, but not in dyslexics. The intercepting line in the box denotes the group median, x represents the mean, while the box denotes data points from the first to third quartiles. The range is represented by the whiskers

Discussion

This study investigated how high-school students with dyslexia and those without differed in HP (using the composite paradigm) and LSB in Chinese character perception in order to understand the mechanism underlying the deficits in Chinese dyslexia. Recent research has suggested that the RH has the flexibility to switch between holistic and part-based representations in visual object recognition, depending on whether the recognition relies on configural or featural information, respectively (Chung et al., 2018a, b; Hsiao & Galmar, 2016). Consistent with this finding, expertise in Chinese character recognition, where featural information plays a more important role, is associated with decreased HP and increased LSB/RH lateralization (Hsiao & Cottrell, 2009; Tso et al., 2014). Accordingly, if reading difficulty in students with dyslexia was associated with inability to de-emphasize configural information unimportant for Chinese character identification, they would show increased HP together with LSB. In contrast, if they showed increased HP together with decreased LSB, it would suggest that their deficit was in the inability to attend to character parts to form appropriate part-based representations in the RH.

Our results showed that participants with dyslexia demonstrated stronger HP and weaker LSB in Chinese character perception than typically developing controls. Thus, they may have deficits in selectively attending to character parts to form appropriate part-based representations in the RH. This finding was consistent with previous studies reporting weaker RH involvement in Chinese character recognition in Chinese dyslexia (Siok et al., 2004). These part-based representations may be important for readers to learn about first-order relations (relative positions) among character parts. In contrast to second-order relations (exact distances) among parts (see Maurer et al., 2002), the knowledge about first-order relations plays an essential role in Chinese character recognition. As Chinese character components are position-specific and often convey specific semantic or phonological information (Hsiao & Shillcock, 2006), the awareness of first-order relations among components in a character, or orthographic awareness, allows readers to efficiently identify the meaning and pronunciation of the character. Consistent with this speculation, here students with dyslexia had a lower performance in lexical decision when compared with that in controls. Indeed, in previous studies, Chinese readers with dyslexia have often been found to have impaired orthographic awareness as compared with controls (Chung & Ho, 2010).

Note that while both students with dyslexia and Limited-writers in Tso et al.’s (2014) study demonstrated increased HP and low character dictation performance, they differed in LSB: Limited-writers showed LSB whereas students with dyslexia did not. This phenomenon suggests different mechanisms underlying their HP. Without adequate writing experience, Limited-writers may have less ability to de-emphasize configural information among features that are unimportant for Chinese characters, leading to increased HP together with LSB. In contrast, since writing practice has been regularly implemented in the curriculum for Chinese literacy in Hong Kong, students with dyslexia may have learned to de-emphasize unimportant configural information but suffered from deficits in visually separating and attending to character components, leading to increased HP with decreased LSB. The current examination helped us to tease apart these two different mechanisms underlying HP. Consistent with this speculation, here we did not observe a significant correlation between HP and dictation performance among the participants (Supplementary Material, Table S1), in contrast to Tso et al.’s (2014) study that suggested a close relationship between HP and writing ability. This phenomenon again suggested different mechanisms underlying HP effects in Chinese character processing. Alternatively, it is possible that both Limited-writers and students with dyslexia had deficits to de-emphasize unimportant configural information, leading to increased HP, and the reduced LSB in students with dyslexia may be related to some other factors affecting the LSB effect, such as deficits in processing first-order relations among components. These possibilities require further investigations.

Our results suggest that interventions that involve enhancing the ability to visually separate and selectively attend to character components may help Chinese readers with dyslexia to develop appropriate representations for character parts and the first-order relations among them. This may in turn enhance orthographic awareness and reading performance. Interventions designed for young students with dyslexia often involve identifying and recognizing individual semantic and phonetic components within Chinese characters, which has been reported to significantly improve their performances in character recognition and writing (Tse, 2011). The weaker LSB and increased HP effects in the students with dyslexia could also be related to the possibility of reduced reading experience due to their reading difficulties. Indeed, recent research has shown that literacy experience modulates neural responses to written words (Dehaene, Cohen, Morais, & Kolinsky, 2015). Thus, adequate literature exposure may also play an important role in the interventions.

Franceschini, Bertoni, Gianesini, Gori, and Facoetti (2017) recently reported reduced global processing in people with dyslexia in Italian using hierarchical letter pattern stimuli (Navon, 1977). This result was consistent with our finding of a reduced LSB effect in Chinese dyslexia. Note, however, that Franceschini et al. (2017) based their study on Italian readers while the current study was on Chinese reading processes. In addition, the task using Navon stimuli measures participants’ ability to attend to either the global form or the local components of the stimuli when all local components have the same identity, whereas the composite paradigm in the current study measures participants’ ability to selectively attend to a stimulus part under the interference from surrounding parts of a different identity. It remains unclear whether English dyslexics would show increased HP effects with English word stimuli or whether Chinese dyslexics would show reduced global processing using Navon stimuli. Franceschini et al.’s (2017) finding suggested that dyslexics in alphabetic languages may have problems integrating letters to form a decodable word, whereas our results showed that dyslexia in Chinese may have impaired part processing in the RH. Together these results suggest that dyslexia may be associated with impaired RH processing in flexibly switching between holistic and part-based representations. This speculation is consistent with recent research suggesting that children with dyslexia may have deficits in coordination between analytic and holistic processing needed for fluent reading (e.g., Lachmann, 2018; Schmitt, Lachmann, & Leeuwen, 2019).

Consistent with the above discussions on the association between dyslexia and impairment in RH processing, children with dyslexia in both alphabetic languages (e.g., Bosse, Tainturier, & Valdois, 2007; Facoetti, et al., 2001; Gabay, Gabay, Henik, Schiff, & Behrmann, 2015; Gabay, Gabay, Schiff, Ashkenazi, & Henik, 2013; Stein & Walsh, 1997) and the Chinese language (e.g., Liu, Pan, & Xu, 2018) were shown to have deficits in visual spatial attention. For example, Bosse et al. (2007) found that many British and French children with dyslexia exhibited deficits in visual attention span, i.e., the number of visual elements that can be processed simultaneously, in addition to phonological disorders. This impairment may influence recognition in other visual domains. Indeed, recent research has reported that individuals with dyslexia in an alphabetic script also showed impaired face processing, suggesting that cognitive processes in different domains may be interdependent (e.g., Behrmann & Plaut, 2012, 2013; Gabay, Dundas, Plaut, & Behrmann, 2017; Sigurdardottir, Arnardottir, Thuridur, Halldorsdottir, & Valgeirsdottir, 2019). Future work will examine whether those with Chinese dyslexia also have deficits in face processing. The inclusion of non-linguistic stimuli such as faces will also help us understand whether the observed LSB and HP effects are related to impaired linguistic processing.

In contrast to Chinese readers with dyslexia, people with prosopagnosia were reported to have reduced LSB (Malaspina et al., 2016) and HP (Avidan et al., 2011) in face perception. These findings suggest that the deficits may be related to difficulties in forming appropriate configural representations in the RH. Note however that recent research has suggested both configural and featural information are important for face recognition (Sandford & Burton, 2014). The optimal strategy may involve the flexibility to switch between the use of configural and featural information (Chan, Chan, Lee, & Hsiao, 2018; Chuk, Crookes, Hayward, Chan, & Hsiao, 2017). Future work may examine HP and LSB in prosopagnosia patients to better understand whether deficits in forming either configural or part-based representations in the RH can lead to face recognition difficulties.

To conclude, here we have shown that Chinese readers with dyslexia had increased HP, decreased LSB, and worse dictation performances on Chinese characters as compared with typically developing controls. This result was in contrast to Limited writers reported in Tso et al. (2014), who showed increased HP and worse dictation performance with a significant LSB, demonstrating two different perceptual mechanisms underlying HP. Recent studies have suggested that the RH can switch between holistic and part-based representations depending on whether the recognition relies on configural or featural information respectively (Chung et al., 2018a, b; Harris & Aguirre, 2010). As Chinese character recognition relies more on featural than configural information, Limited writers’ HP with LSB suggests their difficulties in de-emphasizing configural information unimportant to Chinese character recognition due to inadequate writing experience. In contrast, the stronger HP with reduced LSB in readers with dyslexia suggests their deficits in selectively attending to character parts to form appropriate part-based representations in the RH, which may in turn impair orthographic awareness, i.e., knowledge of first-order relations among components. Thus, secondary school students with dyslexia may still encounter difficulties in reading and writing due to persistent deficits in their literacy-related cognitive abilities, and they may require further support to enhance attention to Chinese character components.