Memory & Cognition

, Volume 46, Issue 1, pp 132–147 | Cite as

Decomposing item-method directed forgetting of emotional pictures: Equivalent costs and no benefits

  • Tracy L. Taylor
  • Chelsea K. Quinlan
  • Kelly C. H. Vullings
Article
  • 65 Downloads

Abstract

Using an item-method directed forgetting task, we presented negative, neutral, and positive photographic pictures, one at a time, each followed by an instruction to remember or forget. We determined that the directed forgetting effect, defined as better subsequent recognition of to-be-remembered (TBR) items than to-be-forgotten (TBF) items, was equivalent across negative, neutral, and positive pictures. To disentangle the underlying costs (i.e., decrease in memory for TBF items) and benefits (i.e., increase in memory for TBR items), we compared recognition memory performance in the directed forgetting task to that of a novel within-subjects remember-all control condition (Experiment 1) and to a between-subjects remember-all control group (Experiment 2). We observed costs without benefits across all three emotions—negative, neutral, and positive—in both experiments. These results demonstrate that equivalent directed forgetting effects for emotional stimuli are not attributable to different underlying component processes. Instead, our results suggest that selection for encoding is accomplished in similar ways, regardless of emotional content.

Keywords

Item-method directed forgetting Intentional forgetting Costs and benefits Pictures Emotion 

References

  1. Anderson, M. C., & Hanslmayr, S. (2014). Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(6), 279–92.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bailey, K., & Chapman, P. (2012). When can we choose to forget? An ERP study into item-method directed forgetting of emotional words. Brain and Cognition, 78(2), 133–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J. (1993). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicit memory tests: A comparison of methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 603–616.Google Scholar
  4. Bastin, C., Feyers, D., Majerus, S., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., . . . Collette, F. (2012). The neural substrates of memory suppression: A FMRI exploration of directed forgetting. PLOS ONE, 7(1), e29905.Google Scholar
  5. Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
  6. Bjork, R. A., Bjork, E. L., & Anderson, M. C. (1998). Varieties of goal-directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 103–137). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Brandt, K. R., Nielsen, M. K., & Holmes, A. (2013). Forgetting emotional and neutral words: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1501, 21–31.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buchanan, T. W., & Lovallo, W. R. (2001). Enhanced memory for emotional material following stress-level cortisol treatment in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(3), 307–317.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cheng, S., Liu, I., Lee, J. R., Hung D. L., & Tzeng, O. J.-L. (2012). Intention forgetting might be more effortful than remembering: An ERP study of item-method directed forgetting. Biological Psychology, 89, 283–292.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cloitre, M., Cancienne, J., Brodsky, B., Dulit, R., & Perry, S. W. (1996). Memory performance among women with parental abuse histories: Enhanced directed forgetting or directed remembering?. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(2), 204-211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cohen, J. D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25, 257–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dewhurst, S. A., & Parry, L. A. (2000). Emotionality, distinctiveness, and recollective experience. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12(4), 541–551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dolcos, F., & Cabeza, R. (2002). Event-related potentials of emotional memory: Encoding pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(3), 252–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2005). Remembering one year later: role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(7), 2626–2631.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dougal, S., & Rotello, C. M. (2007). “Remembering” emotional words is based on response bias, not recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 423–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Elzinga, B. M., de Beurs, E., Sergeant, J. A., Van Dyck, R., & Phaf, R. H. (2000). Dissociative style and directed forgetting. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(3), 279–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Elzinga, B. M., Phaf, R. H., Ardon, A. M., & van Dyck, R. (2003). Directed forgetting between, but not within, dissociative personality states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(2), 237–243.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fawcett, J. M., Lawrence, M. A., & Taylor, T. L. (2016). The representational consequences of intentional forgetting: Impairments to both the probability and fidelity of long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), 56–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fawcett, J. M., & Taylor, T. L. (2008). Forgetting is effortful: Evidence from reaction time probes in an item-method directed forgetting task. Memory & Cognition, 6, 1168–1181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fawcett, J. M., & Taylor, T. L. (2010). Directed forgetting shares mechanisms with attentional withdrawal but not with stop-signal inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 38, 797–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fawcett, J. M., & Taylor, T. L. (2012). The control of working memory resources in intentional forgetting: Evidence from incidental probe word recognition. Acta Psychologica, 139, 84–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gallant, S. N., & Dyson, B. J. (2016). Neural modulation of directed forgetting by valence and arousal: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1648, 306–316.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gallant, S. N., & Yang, L. (2014). Positivity effect in source attributions of arousal-matched emotional and non-emotional words during item-based directed forgetting. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1334.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gardiner, J. M., Gawlik, B., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1994). Maintenance rehearsal affects knowing, not remembering: Elaborative rehearsal affects remembering, not knowing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(1), 107–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Geiselman, R. E., Bjork, R. A., & Fishman, D. L. (1983). Disrupted retrieval in directed forgetting: A link with posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112(1), 58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Goernert, P. N., Corenblum, B., & Otani, H. (2011). Directed forgetting of faces: The role of response criterion. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(10), 1930–1938.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gottlob, L. R., Golding, J. M., & Hauselt, W. J. (2006). Directed forgetting of a single item. The Journal of General Psychology, 133(1), 67–80.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., & Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 2(3), 289–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hauswald, A., & Kissler, J. (2008). Directed forgetting of complex pictures in an item method paradigm. Memory, 16(8), 797–809.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hauswald, A., Schulz, H., Iordanov, T., & Kissler, J. (2010). ERP dynamics underlying successful directed forgetting of neutral but not negative pictures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(4), 450–459.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hourihan, K. L., & MacLeod, C. M. (2008). Directed forgetting meets the production effect: Distinctive processing is resistant to intentional forgetting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(4), 242–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hourihan, K. L., Ozubko, J. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2009). Directed forgetting of visual symbols: Evidence for nonverbal selective rehearsal. Memory & Cognition, 37(8), 1059–1068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hourihan, K. L., & Taylor, T. L. (2006). Cease remembering: Executive control processes in directed forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 1354–1365.Google Scholar
  34. Hsieh, L. T., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., Lee, J. R., & Cheng, S. K. (2009). An event-related potential investigation of the processing of remember/forget cues and item encoding in item-method directed forgetting. Brain Research, 1250, 190–201.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jonides, J., & Mack, R. (1984). On the cost and benefit of cost and benefit. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 29–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory & Cognition, 31(8), 1169–1180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kleinsmith, L. J., & Kaplan, S. (1963). Paired-associate learning as a function of arousal and interpolated interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 190–193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Korfine, L., & Hooley, J. M. (2000). Directed forgetting of emotional stimuli in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 214–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kruschke, J. K., & Liddell, T. M. (2017). The Bayesian new statistics: Hypothesis testing, estimation, meta-analysis, and power analysis from a Bayesian perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–29. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1221-4
  41. LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 54–64.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. LaBar, K. S., & Phelps, E. A. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: Role of the medial temporal lobe in humans. Psychological Science, 9, 527–540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-6, pp. 1–6). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
  44. Lawrence , M. A. (2016). ez: Easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiment (Package Version 4.4-0) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ez
  45. Liang, C. W., Hsu, W. Y., Hung, F. C., Wang, W. T., & Lin, C. H. (2011). Absence of a positive bias in social anxiety: The application of a directed forgetting paradigm. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(2), 204–210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 1–57). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  47. MacLeod, C. M., & Daniels, K. A. (2000). Direct versus indirect tests of memory: Directed forgetting meets the generation effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(2), 354–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Masson, M. E. (2011). A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behavior Research Methods, 43(3), 679–690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Temporal cognition: Connecting subjective time to perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 865–907.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. McNally, R. J., Clancy, S. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2001). Directed forgetting of trauma cues in adults reporting repressed or recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 151–156.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. McNally, R. J., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., & Pitman, R. K. (1998). Directed forgetting of trauma cues in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 596–601.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. McNally, R. J., Otto, M. W., Yap, L., Pollack, M. H., & Hornig, C. D. (1999). Is panic disorder linked to cognitive avoidance of threatening information? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13, 335–348.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Moulds, M. L., & Bryant, R. A. (2002). Directed forgetting in acute stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 175–179.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Nowicka, A., Marchewka, A., Jednorog, K, Tacikowski, P., & Brechmann, A. (2010). Forgetting of emotional information is hard: An fMRI study of directed forgetting. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 539–549.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ochsner, K. N. (2000). Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 242–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Otani, H., Libkuman, T. M., Goernert, P. N., Kato, K., Migita, M., Freehafer, S. E., & Landow, M. P. (2012). Emotion, directed forgetting, and source memory. British Journal of Psychology, 103(3), 343–358.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Paller, K. A. (1990). Recall and stem-completion priming have different electrophysiological correlates and are modified differentially by directed forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1021–1032.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Patrick, R. E., Kiang, M., & Christensen, B. K. (2015). Neurophysiological correlates of emotional directed-forgetting in persons with Schizophrenia: An event-related brain potential study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 612–623.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Paz-Caballero, M. D., Menor, J., & Jiménez, J. M. (2004). Predictive validity of event-related potentials (ERPs) in relation to the directed forgetting effects. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 369–377.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Quinlan, C. K., & Taylor, T. L. (2014). “I never forget a face, but in your case I’ll be glad to make an exception”: Intention forgetting of emotional faces. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 212–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Quinlan, C. K., Taylor, T. L., & Fawcett, J. M. (2010). Directed forgetting: Comparing pictures and words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 41–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
  63. Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Rizio, A. A., & Dennis, N. A. (2013). The neural correlates of cognitive control: Successful remembering and intentional forgetting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 297–312.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sahakyan, L., & Foster, N. L. (2009). Intentional forgetting of actions: Comparison of list-method and item-method directed forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 134–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1064–1072.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Sharot, T., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 294–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Talmi, D. (2013). Enhanced emotional memory: Cognitive and neural mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 430–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Talmi, D., Luk, B. T., McGarry, L. M., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). The contribution of relatedness and distinctiveness to emotionally-enhanced memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 555–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Taylor, T. L. (2005). Inhibition of return following instructions to remember and forget. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 613–629 (Erratum in 58A, 1343).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Taylor, T. L., & Fawcett, J. M. (2011). Larger IOR effects following forget than following remember instructions depend on exogenous attentional withdrawal and target localization. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1790–1814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Taylor, T. L., & Fawcett, J. M. (2012). Does an instruction to forget enhance memory for other presented items? Consciousness & Cognition, 21, 1186–1197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Thompson, K. M., Hamm, J. P., & Taylor, T. L. (2014). Effects of memory instruction on attention and information processing: Further investigation of inhibition of return in item-method directed forgetting. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 322–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Tolin, D. F., Hamlin, C., & Foa, E. B. (2002). Directed forgetting in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Replication and extension. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 793–803.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. van Hooff, J. C., & Ford, R. M. (2011). Remember to forget: ERP evidence for inhibition in an item-method directed forgetting paradigm. Brain Research, 1392, 80–92.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wickham, H. (2011). The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 40, 1–29. Retrieved from http://www.jstatsoft.org/v40/i01/ Google Scholar
  78. Wickham, H. (2017a). stringr: Simple, consistent wrappers for common string operations (Package Version 1.2.0) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=stringr
  79. Wickham, H. (2017b). tidyr: Easily tidy data with spread() and gather() functions (R Package Version 0.6.3) [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=tidyr
  80. Wickham, H., & Francois, R. (2016). dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation (R Package Version 0.5.0) [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr
  81. Wilhelm, S., McNally, R. J., Baer, L., & Florin, I. (1996). Directed forgetting in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 633–641.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Wong, A. C., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Depressive rumination and directed forgetting: An examination of encoding style. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Wylie, G. R., Foxe, J. J., & Taylor, T. L. (2008). Forgetting as an active process: An fMRI investigation of item-method directed forgetting. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 670–682.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Yang, W., Liu, P., Cui, Q., Wei, D., Li, W., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. (2013). Directed forgetting of negative self-referential information is difficult: An fMRI study. PLOS ONE, 8(10), e75190.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Yang, W., Liu, P., Xiao, X., Li, X., Zeng, C., Qui, J., & Zhang, Q. (2012). Different neural substrates underlying directed forgetting for negative and neutral images: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1441, 53–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Tracy L. Taylor
    • 1
  • Chelsea K. Quinlan
    • 1
  • Kelly C. H. Vullings
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceDalhousie UniversityHalifaxCanada

Personalised recommendations