Background

Identifying the scents of individual persons using specially trained canines is a well-known forensic method (Settle et al. 1994; Schoon 1996; Schoon 2005) frequently used in criminal investigations. The results of these identifications are generally understood to be subjective and are legally accepted as corroborating evidence (Marchal et al. 2016; Tomaszewski and Girdwoyn 2006) only in some countries (the USA, Spain, France, Russia, Czechia, Poland, Slovakia, etc.). Hence, the development of an objective, instrumental method that will enable comparative scent identifications is highly desirable. Clearly, such an analytical technique would be more credible from forensic and judicial perspectives. To develop such an instrumental method, profound chemical understanding of the human scent signature is absolutely necessary. However, this knowledge has so far been lacking.

From a chemical perspective, the human scent is a complicated mixture of several thousand compounds that significantly differ (Curran et al. 2010; Curran et al. 2005; Doležal et al. 2017; Gallagher et al. 2008) in concentration and chemical properties. The human scent can usually be classified into primary, secondary, and tertiary categories (Curran et al. 2005). The primary scent category includes compounds that are assumed to be genetically conditioned. Their relative concentrations are approximately stable over time regardless of environmental factors, diet, weather, humidity, visceral state, emotional state, illness, menstrual cycle, medication, etc. On the other hand, the skin scent compounds, whose amounts depend on these internal and external factors, are in the secondary category. Finally, tertiary scents originate from the external environment, e.g., from cosmetics used, scents of the workplace, gasoline, smoking, scents of other people, and domestic animals.

From the above definitions of the scent categories, the active human scent signature should comprise the primary scent compounds. However, these primary molecules have not yet been unambiguously recognized; nevertheless, many papers (Curran et al. 2010; Curran et al. 2005; Gallagher et al. 2008; Prada et al. 2015) assume that the human scent signature is formed primarily by volatile organic compounds (VOC). In principle, the designation “volatile organic compounds” does not differentiate between highly or poorly volatile compounds. However, the majority of previous chemical analyses of human scent samples (Cui et al. 2009; Curran et al. 2010; Curran et al. 2005; Kusano et al. 2011; Pandey and Kim 2011) implicitly presumed that the highly (and perhaps also moderately) volatile compounds are the most important for human scent signatures. This assumption follows from the analytical chemistry methods that are typically used in these chemical scent analyses. Headspace solid-phase microextraction gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (HS-SPME–GC/MS), which was frequently used for the chemical analyses of human scent samples (Cui et al. 2009; Curran et al. 2010; Curran et al. 2005; Kusano et al. 2011; Pandey and Kim 2011), discriminates against poorly volatile compounds (Pandey and Kim 2011). Nevertheless, from a forensic point of view, these poorly volatile compounds can be extremely important since the collection of a scent trace from a crime scene can be delayed by several days to a couple of weeks, but the scent signature is still recognizable by trained canines.

The essential aim of this study was to specify the group of chemical compounds that specially trained canines use for the scent identification of persons. Therefore, scent samples from 11 volunteers were split into three disjointed fractions using a gas chromatograph connected with a preparative fraction collector (GC-PFC). These scent fractions were subjected to a scent identification line-up using specially trained canines to determine which fraction contains the active scent signature. The volunteers were chosen to be the same type, therefore the same sex, age (approximately 22 years old), and the same European ethnicity, to eliminate possible effects of the “class identification” in our identification line-up procedure.

Methods

Preparation of human scent samples

The scent samples were taken from the volunteer’s palms using 40 chemical cleaned glass beads as a sorbent (Bernier et al. 1999; Doležal et al. 2017). This sorption material (soda-lime glass, 4.86 ± 0.17 mm in diameter) was used for scent collection because, compared with the fabrics typically used (sterile gauze, gauze pads, or a blend of cotton, rayon, polyester, wool, etc.), it can be cleaned significantly better (Prada et al. 2010). The glass beads were thoroughly purified first in a mixture of dichromate-sulfuric acid and then in ultrapure water, ethanol (> 99.8%, PENTA, Ltd., Czech Republic), and hexane (≥ 97.0%, Sigma-Aldrich, Merck, Saint Louis, USA), and finally, they were dried in an oven at 210 °C for 5 h. The purified beads were stored in a desiccator until sampling. The purity of the cleaned glass beads was tested by GC/MS. The purification details are described in our previous study (Doležal et al. 2017).

Female volunteers were chosen for this primary study since their scents are usually more recognizable by the specially trained canines than male scents (Jezierski et al. 2012).

First, the volunteers intensively washed their palms with a special soap (non-perfumed, Amadeus Neutral soap, CORMEN, Czech Republic) and warm water. The volunteers’ palms were then allowed to dry in the air for 10 min. Subsequently, those volunteers rolled 40 chemically purified beads between their open palms for 10 min. After that, samples were extracted from the beads with the adsorbed scent two times with 0.7 ml hexane. The extracted samples free evaporated from a volume of 1400 μl to 200 μl. These scent samples were stored in a refrigerator (below 7 °C, 44.6 °F or 280.2 K) for up to 1 week before the chromatographic experiments were performed.

Preparations of scent fractions using a preparative GC

An Agilent 6890 GC/FID with an automatic Preparative Fraction Collector (GC-PFC, GERSTEL GmbH & Co.KG, Mülheim an der Ruhr, Germany) was used for the GC-preparation of the scent fractions. The preparative column used was an Rxi®-5 ms (length 30 m, ID 0.53 mm, df 1.5 μm; Restek Corporation, Bellefonte, USA) with He (5.5) as the carrier gas (constant flow rate: 6.0 ml/min). The total run time was 46 min. The PTV (programmable temperature vaporization) injection was performed in the splitless mode (SSL). The GC temperature program and the settings of the SSL-PTV and the fraction collector (the heated box) are listed in Table 1.

Table 1 GC-PFC parameters

All the human scent samples were split into three fractions (see Table 2 and Fig. 1). The first fraction (to decanoic acid) are VOCs, the second fraction contains moderately volatile compounds, and the third “after squalene” fraction contains poorly volatile compounds such as fatty acid esters (Doležal et al. 2017). The retention indices were calibrated using n-alkanes (C7–C40, Sigma-Aldrich, Merck, Germany). Seven runs of analyses with 5-μl injection volumes were carried out for each sample, which means that a total volume of 35 μl was injected for each sample. The Grob Test Mix (Sigma-Aldrich, Merck, Germany) was injected between the scent samples of different volunteers, and the efficacies of the individual peaks were monitored.

Table 2 Fraction retention times and corresponding retention indices (RI)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Chromatogram of a human scent sample with the indicated intervals of the fractions

The fractions were captured in 1-μl coiled glass traps that were immersed in the cooling box (at a temperature of − 50 °C, cooled by liquid N2). The glass traps were stored in cleaned jars with twist-off lids, which afterwards were used for the starting scents (SS) in the line-up analyses with the trained canines.

Human scent identification by specially trained canines

All 33 fractions of human scent samples were obtained from the 11 female volunteers. These fractioned scent samples were subjected to scent identification line-ups as starting scents (Vyplelová et al. 2014) using six canines (female German Shepherds) that were specially trained for the scent identification line-up method. These starting scent samples were prepared from the volunteer’s scent samples, which were obtained either from the 1st fraction (highly volatile) or from the 2nd fraction (moderately volatile) or from the 3rd fraction (poorly volatile compounds). The starting samples which split into these three fractions were compared with the “full” (i.e., non-fractioned) scent samples in the line-up (see diagram in Fig. 2, lower right side) where the target scent (TS) consisted of only one sample, and the remaining samples were distracting scent (DS) samples. These scent samples (DS) were collected from other volunteers on sterilized fabric sorbents (cotton-based) that are ordinarily used by the Czech Republic’s police. To obtain reliable results, the distracting scent samples were taken from volunteers of the same sex and similar age to avoid the “odd-man out” effect (Schoon and Haak 2002).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Diagram of the collection, preparation, and testing of the human scent samples. The symbol SS denotes a starting scent represented by one fraction, TS denotes a “full” target scent sample (see text above), and AT denotes a scent sample used for an “attractivity test” (see text). In the diagram, volunteer* denotes one of the 11 volunteers whose scent samples were fractioned. The designation volunteers** refers to the group of 20 other volunteers who provided their scent samples. These samples were used as the distracting scents (DSs)

Before the line-up scent identification, all the canines were subjected to attractivity tests (AT) (see Fig. 2). The sense and details of the attractivity test as well as the standard human scent identification line-up procedure are described in many studies (e.g., (Pinc et al. 2011; Santariová et al. 2016; Schoon and Haak 2002; Vyplelová et al. 2014).

The canines were allowed to sniff the fractioned starting scent samples (SS) and then were sent to search for the target scent (TS) in the line-up (Fig. 2). The canines indicated an identified target scent (TS) by sitting or lying down. All the scent identifications were performed consistently in a double-blind routine, which means that neither the dog handler nor anyone else in the experimental room had any information on the position of the target scent (TS) in the line-up. Each of the scent fractions was tested by the canine three times. The positions of the scent samples (TS and DS) in the lines were randomly (using dice) interchanged by another assistant.

If the canine did not identify the starting scent (random selected fraction from the scent sample of person 1) with the target scent in the line-up, the next fraction (second or third) from the same person was also tested by the same canine. For the opposite case, this canine was replaced by another canine and this one tested the next scent fractions from person 1 (see Tables 3 and 4) to avoid misleading identifications due to the memory effect (Schoon and Haak 2002).

Table 3 Results of the human scent identification line-ups performed using six canines
Table 4 Success rate of the particular scent fractions in the scent identification line-up

The canines were not rewarded for positive indications after the first and second line-up identifications (see the diagram in Fig. 2) to not encourage the canine to indicate the same sample again.

All of the canines used in the experiments were specially trained for human scent identification. Their work experience ranged from 3 to 7 years.

Statistical evaluations

The results from the scent identification line-ups listed in Tables 3 and 4 were evaluated using Fisher’s exact test and Pearson’s chi-squared test by means of SAS 9.4. software (SAS Institute Inc. 2013. Base SAS® 9.4 [computer program]. Guide: Statistical Procedures). Pearson’s chi-squared test with 2 degrees of freedom was used for an association assessment between the different scent fractions and the correct canine identifications.

Results and discussion

All of the results obtained from the human scent identifications using specially trained canines are summarized in Table 3. In these line-up identifications, the trained canines compared the fractioned starting scent (SS) samples with the “full” target samples (TS) and other distracting scents (DS) in the line-up.

Before scent identification, the same scent samples were analyzed by comprehensive two-dimensional gas-chromatography–time of flight mass spectrometry (GCxGC–TOFMS) and the results were discussed in our study (Doležal et al. 2017). The partition of the individual fractions was based on this previous research. In this manner, the following groups of compounds were obtained:

The first scent fraction (see Fig. 1) contains highly volatile molecules (Dormont et al. 2013; Gallagher et al. 2008; Prada et al. 2015), e.g., volatile hydrocarbons, alcohols, aldehydes, aromatic compounds, and organic acid esters (up to approximately 7 carbons). The second fraction contains moderately volatile compounds such as hydrocarbons, alcohols, acids with longer chains, and fatty acid esters with shorter carbon chains. The third fraction contains poorly volatile compounds such as fatty acid esters, mostly with C16–C18, different types of sterols, and certain vitamins.

The results obtained using the trained canines provided several remarkable observations. The results in Table 3 prove that a single human scent trace may contain several groups of molecules that make individual scent identifications possible. There are several scent signatures in one scent trace. In this paper, this phenomenon has been named the multiplicity of the human scent signature.

In addition to this finding, the results also show that the poorly volatile organic compounds (the 3rd fraction) are analogously as important for scent identification as the highly and moderately volatile compounds (the 1st and 2nd fractions). In our experiments, our specially trained canines most often preferred the poorly volatile molecules in the scent traces for the identification of people. Tables 3 and 4, which summarize the success rates for the scent identification line-ups for all the scent fractions, support this observation.

The poorly volatile fraction (the 3rd fraction) resulted in successful individual identification 28 times out of 33 attempts, thus an 85% success rate. The middle fraction (the 2nd fraction) resulted in successful identification 19 times out of 33 (58%) and the highly volatile fraction (the 1st fraction) resulted in successful identification in 11 attempts out of 33 (30%).

The statistical evaluations showed a significant association between the correct identification line-ups by canines and the particular scent fractions. There are highly significant differences in scent identification success according to the different scent fractions (P < 0.01). This observation is also supported by Pearson’s chi-squared tests that unambiguously show that the correct identification line-ups depend with 99% certainty on the scent fraction used (P < 0.01). If all the scent identification results were taken together, the 3rd fraction was most often matched correctly (chi-square = 18.07, 2 degrees of freedom, P < 0.001). Considering the fraction couples separately, the 2nd and 3rd fractions were significantly more frequently correctly identified than the 1st fraction. A highly significant difference was found between the scent identification successes of the 1st and the 3rd fractions (chi-square = 11.20, P < 0.01). On the other hand, a less significant difference was found between the success rates of the 2nd and the 3rd fractions (chi-square = 4.13, P < 0.05), followed by the 1st and the 2nd fractions (chi-square = 2.71, P < 0.05).

Conclusions

During this study, 198 “full” scent samples were collected from volunteers and used in line-up identifications as target (TS) and distracting scent (DS) samples (see diagram, Fig. 2). In addition, another 11 samples were collected on glass beads (see the “Methods” section). These scent samples were split into three disjointed fractions; in sum, 33 fractioned scent samples were prepared (3 for each volunteer), which were used as the starting samples (SS) in the line-up procedures. In total, 99 scent identifications were performed with 58 positive identifications (+, see Tables 3 and 4), and the remaining scent tests were performed without any identification (−). These results mean that the canines did not carry out any false identification!

The comparisons of the particular disjointed fractions with the “full” target scents (TS) and distracting scents (DS) in the line-ups using the trained canines provided two important findings.

The first finding is the observation that the human scent trace contains more than one group of compounds that allow for the individual scent identification of persons. This means that several human scent signatures co-exist in one scent trace. The canines may be performing these individual scent identifications in an analogous way to people identifying each other by sight. A human is able to identify a person based on his or her face, body posture, style of movement, hairstyle, etc.

Analogously, a canine can use different groups of molecules for the identification of individual people depending on various conditions. In both these cases, the different recognition methods (the different scent signatures) are variable in its reliability and durability.

The second principal observation is that the poorly volatile compounds in the human scent trace play a very important role in the scent identification procedure. Our experiments imply that their role is even more important than that of the highly and moderately volatile organic compounds. These volatile organic compounds are assumed to be the essential constituents of the human scent trace (Curran et al. 2010; Prada et al. 2015), and they are assumed to form the human scent signature. However, the obtained results could be influenced by the temperature program (110–320 °C for 46 min) during the fraction preparations by GC-PFC. Such conditions could cause the degradation of some thermolabile compounds and thus influence the molecular compositions of the individual fractions. This degradation may be the main factor reducing the success rates of the first and second fractions. Nevertheless, our experiments unambiguously confirm the multiplicity of the human scent signature and the significance of the poorly volatile compounds (as fatty acid esters) in the scent identification method.

We hope that the results of this study will open a discussion about the multiplicity of the human scent signature, primary scent molecules, optimal canine training, and in general, the scent identification method.