Skip to main content
Log in

Perceptual Modification of the Built Environment to Influence Behavior Associated with Physical Activity: Quasi-Experimental Field Studies of a Stair Banister Illusion

  • Original Research Article
  • Published:
Sports Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

Re-engineering the built environment to influence behaviors associated with physical activity potentially provides an opportunity to promote healthier lifestyles at a population level. Here we present evidence from two quasi-experimental field studies in which we tested a novel, yet deceptively simple, intervention designed to alter perception of, and walking behavior associated with, stairs in an urban area.

Objectives

Our objectives were to examine whether adjusting a stair banister has an influence on perceptions of stair steepness or on walking behavior when approaching the stairs.

Methods

In study 1, we asked participants (n = 143) to visually estimate the steepness of a set of stairs viewed from the top, when the stair banister was adjusted so that it converged with or diverged from the stairs (± 1.91°) or remained neutral (± 0°). In study 2, the walking behavior of participants (n = 36) was filmed as they approached the stairs to descend, unaware of whether the banister converged, diverged, or was neutral.

Results

In study 1, participants estimated the stairs to be steeper if the banister diverged from, rather than converged with, the stairs. The effect was greater when participants were unaware of the adjustment. In study 2, walking speed was significantly slower when the banister diverged from, rather than converged with, the stairs.

Conclusions

These findings encourage us to speculate about the potential to economically re-engineer features of the built environment to provide opportunities for action (affordances) that invite physical activity behavior or even promote safer navigation of the environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For related arguments, see discussions of evolved navigation theory [16] and error management theory [17].

  2. Order of banister adjustment was not counterbalanced, but there is no reason to believe that the day of the week upon which adjustments occurred (Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday) influenced sampling or responses. Participants who had completed an estimate on a previous day were not tested.

  3. To avoid drawing the attention of participants to the banister–stairs relationship, participants had to complete the unaware condition first, so it was not possible to counterbalance aware/unaware conditions.

  4. Both age and sex have been shown to influence perceptions of stairs/hills [12, 13, 15, 18].

  5. During debriefing, no student indicated awareness that the banister had been adjusted.

  6. Estimation data for one participant was unavailable, so the sample therefore included ten females and 22 males.

  7. Participants indicated the size of the aperture created by a Ponzo illusion with the thumb and index finger (automated, low conscious) or with the thumb and ring finger (awkward, high conscious).

References

  1. Brymer E, Davids K. Designing environments to enhance physical and psychological benefits of physical activity: a multidisciplinary perspective. Sports Med. 2016;46:925–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Frank LD, Sallis JF, Saelens BE, et al. The development of a walkability index: application to the neighborhood quality of life study. Br J Sports Med. 2010;44:924–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Giles-Corti B, Vernez Moudon A, Reis R, et al. City planning and population health: a global challenge. Lancet. 2016;388:2912–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Moura F, Cambra P, Goncalves AB. Measuring walkability for distinct pedestrian groups with a participatory assessment method: a case study in Lisbon. Landsc Urban Plan. 2017;157:282–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Gibson JJ. The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Adolph KE, Eppler MA, Gibson EJ. Crawling versus walking infants’ perception of affordances for locomotion over sloping surfaces. Child Dev. 1993;64:1158–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Fajen BR. The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2005;31:1107–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Lee DN, Lishman R. Visual control of locomotion. Scand J Psychol. 1977;18:224–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Warren WH. Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10:683–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Proffitt DR. Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2006;1:110–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Witt JK, Linkenauger S, Wickens C. Action-specific effects in perception and their potential applications. J Appl Res Mem Cogn. 2016;5:69–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Bhalla M, Proffitt DR. Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1999;25:1076–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Proffitt DR, Bhalla M, Gossweiler R, et al. Perceiving geographical slant. Psychon Bull Rev. 1995;2:409–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Stefanucci JK, Proffitt DR, Clore G, Parekh N. Skating down a steeper slope: Fear influences the perception of geographical slant. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2008;37:321–3.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Taylor-Covill GAH, Eves FF. When what we need influences what we see: choice of energetic replenishment is linked with perceived steepness. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2014;40:915–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Jackson RE, Cormack LK. Evolved navigation theory and the descent illusion. Percept Psychophys. 2007;69:353–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Haselton MG, Buss DM. Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78:81–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Eves FF, Thorpe KS, Lewis A, et al. Does perceived steepness deter stair climbing when an alternative is available? Psychon Bull Rev. 2014;21:637–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Coren S, Girgus J. Seeing is deceiving: the psychology of visual illusions. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Goodale M, Milner AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 1992;15:20–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Milner AD, Goodale M. Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychology. 2008;46:774–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Aglioti S, DeSouza JFX, Goodale M. Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Curr Biol. 1995;5:679–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Chauvel G, Wulf G, Maquestiaux F. Visual illusions can facilitate sport skill learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2015;22:717–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Denton GG. The influence of visual pattern on perceived speed. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1980;9:393–402.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Gray R. Being selective at the plate: processing dependence between perceptual variables relates to hitting goals and performance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2013;39:1124–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Witt JK, Linkenauger SA, Proffitt DR. Get me out of this slump! Visual illusions improve sports performance. Psychol Sci. 2012;23:397–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Wood G, Vine S, Wilson M. The impact of visual illusions on perception, action planning, and motor performance. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2013;75:830–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Gregory RL. Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling. Nature. 1963;199:678–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Weintraub DJ, Krantz DH, Olson TP. The Poggendorff illusion: consider all the angles. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1980;6:718–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Westheimer G. Illusions in the spatial sense of the eye: geometrical-optical illusions and the neural representation of space. Vis Res. 2008;48:2128–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Peng J, Fey NP, Kuiken TA, et al. Anticipatory kinematics and muscle activity preceding transitions from level-ground walking to stair ascent and descent. J Biomech. 2016;49:528–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Glover SR, Dixon P. Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001;27:560–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Elliott DB, Vale A, Whitaker D, et al. Does my step look big in this? A visual illusion leads to safer stepping behaviour. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4577. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004577.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Rhea CK, Rietdyk S, Haddad JM. Locomotor adaptation versus perceptual adaptation when stepping over an obstacle with a height illusion. PLoS One. 2010;5(7):e11544. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011544.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Wong T, Masters RSW, Maxwell JP, Abernethy BA. Reinvestment and falls in community-dwelling older adults. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2008;22:410–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Masters RSW, Pall HS, MacMahon KMA, Eves FF. Duration of Parkinson disease is associated with an increased propensity for ‘reinvestment’. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2007;21:123–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Orrell A, Masters RSW, Eves FF. Reinvestment and movement disruption following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009;23:177–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. The theory of reinvestment. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2008;1:160–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Gonzalez CLR, Ganel T, Whitwell RL, Morrissey B, Goodale MA. Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: Sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychology. 2008;46:624–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Hamel KA, Okita N, Higginson JS, et al. Foot clearance during stair descent: effects of age and illumination. Gait Posture. 2005;21:135–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Zietz D, Johannsen L, Hollands M. Stepping characteristics and centre of mass control during stair descent: Effects of age, fall risk and visual factors. Gait Posture. 2011;34:279–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

No sources of funding were used to assist with preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rich Masters.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Conflict of interest

Rich Masters, Catherine Capio, Jamie Poolton, and Liis Uiga have no conflicts of interest associated with the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Masters, R., Capio, C., Poolton, J. et al. Perceptual Modification of the Built Environment to Influence Behavior Associated with Physical Activity: Quasi-Experimental Field Studies of a Stair Banister Illusion. Sports Med 48, 1505–1511 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-018-0869-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-018-0869-5

Navigation