Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The social, educational and cognitive factors of success in the first year of university: A case study

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
International Review of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The main objective of this study, which evaluated a sample of first-year students enrolled at the University of Burgundy, France, in 2010–2011, is to understand the factors determining success in the first year of university. The originality of this research lies in the inclusion of specific indicators of students’ skills when they start university within the explanatory models of educational achievement. These indicators include measures of academic performance (written comprehension skills) and cognitive abilities. While the impact of cognitive abilities on educational success has been examined at primary level in France, the present study is among the first to do this at higher education level, with the additional consideration of students’ educational and social backgrounds. The results show the significant impact of educational background (repeated years, type of baccalaureate and baccalaureate grade) on success. The researchers also found that written comprehension skills and cognitive abilities alone play a limited role in explaining success, since the impacts of these variables are apparent throughout a student’s educational career (and not just in higher education). Another finding was that subject choice based on specific career aspirations is an important factor associated with success – a significant insight which qualifies the impact of educational background.

Résumé

Facteurs sociaux, éducatifs et cognitifs de succès pendant la première année universitaire : étude de cas – La présente étude, qui évalue un échantillon d’étudiants inscrits en première année 2010-2011 à l’université de Bourgogne (France), a pour objet de cerner les facteurs déterminant la réussite de la première année universitaire. L’originalité de cette recherche réside dans la prise en compte, dans le cadre des modèles explicatifs du niveau d’instruction, d’indicateurs spécifiques sur les compétences que possèdent les nouveaux étudiants. Ces indicateurs comprennent la mesure des performances intellectuelles (compréhension écrite) et des capacités cognitives. Si l’impact des capacités cognitives sur la réussite scolaire est examiné en France au niveau primaire, la présente étude est une des premières à en faire de même au niveau supérieur, en intégrant en outre le contexte éducatif et social des étudiants. Les résultats révèlent l’impact significatif sur la réussite du contexte éducatif (années de redoublement, type de baccalauréat et note obtenue à celui-ci). Les chercheurs constatent en outre que la compréhension écrite et les capacités cognitives jouent à elles seules un rôle limité dans l’explication de la réussite, puisque l’impact de ces variables est manifeste tout au long du parcours éducatif (et pas seulement au niveau supérieur). Il apparaît par ailleurs que le choix de la discipline fondé sur des aspirations concrètes de carrière est un facteur important de succès, et constitue une découverte importante qui relativise l’impact du contexte éducatif.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. All translations from French sources such as this one were prepared by the authors of this paper.

  2. Students in France complete their secondary school cycle by passing a baccalaureate diploma exam (baccalauréat, often abbreviated bac). There are three kinds of bac, namely (1) the baccalauréat général [general baccalaureate, with three subject streams: S (sciences), ES (economic and social studies) and L (literary studies); (2) the baccalauréat technologique [technical baccalaureate, with seven streams: STG (management), STI (industrial studies), STL (scientific an technological laboratory studies), STSS (health and social studies), STAV (agronomy), TMD (music and dance) and Hôtellerie (hotel management)] and (3) the baccalauréat professionnel (vocational baccalaureate) with several streams.

  3. For more information (in English) about CEREQ, see http://www.cereq.fr/index.php/themes/Version-anglaise/A-public-pole-of-expertise.

  4. For more information about OVE, see http://www.ove-national.education.fr/the-ove/presentation.

  5. The DALF test (Diplôme Approfondi en Langue Française) is an advanced-level French language proficiency test. For more information see http://www.ciep.fr/en/delf-prim-delf-delf-junior-version-dalf-diplome-detudes-langue-francaise-diplome-approfondi-langue/dalf-diplome-approfondi-langue-francaise.

  6. The Time-Based Resource Sharing (TBRS) model “accounts for working memory spans in adults […]. [It] assumes that both components (i.e., processing and maintenance) of the main working memory tasks require attention and that memory traces decay as soon as attention is switched away” (Barouillet et al. 2004, p. 83).

  7. American psychologist Michael Posner is known for his research on the role of attention in high-level human tasks such as visual search, reading and number processing.

  8. Originally developed by English psychologist John C. Raven in 1936, this test consists of 60 nonverbal multiple-choice questions designed to measure a person’s reasoning ability (see http://www.raventest.net/).

  9. Guy de Maupassant (1850–1893) is especially renowned for his cleverly-plotted short stories, but he also wrote a number of novels, poems and some travel literature.

  10. The pass mark for the baccalauréat is at 10 out of 20 points. Above that, there are three levels of honours: A result of 12–13.99 points will earn a mention assez bien (honours); 14–15.99 points a mention bien (pass with merit); and 16–20 points a mention très bien (distinction).

  11. Serial reaction time (SRT) investigates cognitive and biological principles of (implicit) learning and memory during a series of repetitive tasks. Complex reaction time (CRT) looks at how people make several decisions about when and how to respond when they are set one complex task.

  12. Model 10 is not presented.

References

  • Astin, A. W. (1984). Student involvement: A developmental theory for higher education. Journal of College Student Personnel, 25(4), 297–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 133(1), 83–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe, E., & Camos, V. (2007). Time and cognitive load in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33(3), 570–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., Camos, V., Morlaix, S., & Suchaut, B. (2008). Progressions scolaires, mémoire de travail et origine sociale: quels liens à l’école élémentaire? Revue Française de Pédagogie, 162, 5–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaupère, N., & Boudesseul, G. (Eds) (2009). Sortir sans diplôme de l’Université. Comprendre les parcours d’étudiants “décrocheurs”. Collection études & recherches. Paris: La Documentation Française.

  • Beduwe, C., & Giret, J.-F. (2004). Le travail en cours d’études a-t-il une valeur professionnelle? Economie et Statistiques, 378–379, 55–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borras, I. (2011). Le tutorat à l’université: peut-on forcer les étudiants à la réussite? Bref du CEREQ, 290, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulon, A. (1997). Le métier d’étudiant: l’entrée dans la vie universitaire. Paris: PUF.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danner, M. (2000). A qui profite le tutorat mis en place dans le premier cycle universitaire? Les sciences de l’éducation pour l’ère nouvelle, 33(1), 25–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duru-Bellat, M. (1995). Des tentatives de prédiction aux écueils de la prévention en matière d’échec scolaire en première année d’université. Savoir Education Formation, 3, 399–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frickey, A., & Primon, J. L. (2002). Jeunes issus de l’immigration: les diplômes de l’enseignement supérieur ne garantissent pas un égal accès au marché du travail. Formation Emploi, 79, 19–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huteau, M., & Lautrey, J. (1999). Evaluer l’intelligence. Psychométrie cognitive. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France (PUF).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1987). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 101–175). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepine, R., Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2005). What makes working memory spans so predictive of high-level cognition? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(1), 165–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MESR (Ministère de l’éducation nationale, de l’enseignement supérieur et de la recherche) (2010). Repères et Références Statistiques sur les enseignements, la formation et la recherche. Edition 2010. Paris: Ministère de l’éducation nationale, de l’enseignement supérieur et de la recherché (MESR) et Direction de l’évaluation, de la prospective et de la performance (DEPP).

  • Michaut, C. (2003). L’efficacité des dispositifs d’aide aux étudiants dans les universités. Recherche et Formation, 43, 101–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prouteau, D. (2009). Parcours et réussite en licence des inscrits en L1 en 2004. Note d’information de la DPD, 23, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romainville, M. (2000). L’échec dans l’université de masse. Paris: L’Harmattan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankland, R. (2009). Pédagogies nouvelles et compétences psychosociales. Paris: L’Harmattan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinto, V. (1993). Leaving college: Rethinking the causes and cures of student attrition (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vergauwe, E., Camos, V., & Barrouillet, P. (2014). The effect of storage on processing: How is information maintained in working memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(4), 1072–1095.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sophie Morlaix.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Morlaix, S., Suchaut, B. The social, educational and cognitive factors of success in the first year of university: A case study. Int Rev Educ 60, 841–862 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11159-014-9459-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11159-014-9459-4

Keywords

Navigation