The challenge and the opportunity of lexical inferencing in language minority students
Abstract
Lexical inferencing from text is a powerful tool for vocabulary and reading comprehension enhancement. Lexical inferencing relies on the pre-requisite skills of reading and existing vocabulary, and is also linked to non-verbal inferencing abilities and reading comprehension. In this study, we examined whether Fifth-grade Russian-speaking language minority (LM) students might exhibit reduced lexical inferencing abilities in comparison to their native Hebrew-speaking (NH) peers, due to their reduced proficiency in the societal language. Participants completed a measure of lexical inferencing during text reading, and measures of underlying skills, including vocabulary, word reading accuracy, reading comprehension and non-verbal inferencing. As a group, LM students demonstrated comparable lexical inferencing abilities to those of their NH peers despite significantly lower vocabulary knowledge in vocabulary. Two explanations are suggested; first, although LM students had reduced vocabulary, they were nonetheless above the vocabulary threshold required for text comprehension. Second, the regression analyses revealed that non-verbal inferencing explained unique variance only in the LM group, demonstrating that they recruited language-external resources to support lexical inferencing. The current results show that lexical inferencing can serve as a powerful tool for promoting reading comprehension and vocabulary, domains that are points of weakness for language minority students.
Keywords
Language minority Lexical inferencing Reading comprehension VocabularyNotes
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Shira Bleicher, Moran Hatan and Ina Kandelis for research assistance and Nashchon Korem for help in data analyses. The current study was partly supported by the Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Study of Learning Disabilities.
References
- August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Bengeleil, N., & Paribakht, T. (2004). L2 reading proficiency and lexical inferencing by university EFL learners. Canadian Modern Language Review, 61(2), 225–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1982). Test of nonverbal intelligence: A language-free measure of cognitive ability [Form A]. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
- Bryant, B. R., & Wiederholt, J. L. (1991). Gray oral reading tests-diagnostic. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
- Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., Barnes, M. A., & Bryant, P. E. (2001). Comprehension skill, inference-making ability, and their relation to knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 29(6), 850–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Elbro, C. (2003). The ability to learn new word meanings from context by school-age children with and without language comprehension difficulties. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 681–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(2), 264–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- De Bot, K., Paribakht, T. S., & Wesche, M. B. (1997). Toward a lexical processing model for the study of second language vocabulary acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(3), 309–329.Google Scholar
- Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second- language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), 78–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunn, L. M. (1965). Expanded manual for the Peabody picture vocabulary test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
- Elgort, I., Perfetti, C. A., Rickles, B., & Stafura, J. Z. (2015). Contextual learning of L2 word meanings: second language proficiency modulates behavioural and event-related brain potential (ERP) indicators of learning. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(5), 506–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Elgort, I., & Warren, P. (2014). L2 vocabulary learning from reading: Explicit and tacit lexical knowledge and the role of learner and item variables. Language Learning, 64(2), 365–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Farnia, F., & Geva, E. (2011). Cognitive correlates of vocabulary growth in English language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(4), 711–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Farnia, F., & Geva, E. (2013). Growth and predictors of change in English language learners’ reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(4), 389–421.Google Scholar
- Geva, E. (2006). Second-language oral proficiency and second-language literacy. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 123–139). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Geva, E., Galili, K., Katzir, T., & Shany, M. (2017). Learning novel words by ear or by eye? An advantage for lexical inferencing in listening versus reading narratives in fourth grade. Reading and Writing, 30(9), 1917–1944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Geva, E., & Massey-Garrison, A. (2013). A comparison of the language skills of ELLs and monolinguals who are poor decoders, poor comprehenders, or normal readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 387–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Geva, E., & Wiener, J. (2015). Psychological assessment of culturally and linguistically diverse children and adolescents. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
- Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Haastrup, K. (1991). Lexical inferencing procedures, or, talking about words: Receptive procedures in foreign language learning with special reference to English (Vol. 14). Germany, Tübingen: Günter Narr Verlag.Google Scholar
- Henriksen, B. (1999). Three dimensions of vocabulary development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(2), 303–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Heppt, B., Haag, N., Böhme, K., & Stanat, P. (2015). The role of academic-language features for reading comprehension of language-minority students and students from low-SES families. Reading Research Quarterly, 50(1), 61–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hoover, A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hsueh-Chao, M. H., & Nation, P. (2000). Unknown vocabulary density and reading comprehension. Reading in a Foreign Language, 13(1), 403–430.Google Scholar
- Jean, M., & Geva, E. (2009). The development of vocabulary in English as a second language children and its role in predicting word recognition ability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 153–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jenkins, J. R., & Dixon, R. (1983). Vocabulary learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8(3), 237–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kame’enui, E. J., & Baumann, J. F. (2012). Vocabulary instruction: Research to practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
- Kavé, G. (2005). Standardization and norms for a Hebrew naming test. Brain and Language, 92(2), 204–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kavé, G. (2006). The development of naming and word fluency: Evidence from Hebrew-speaking children between ages 8 and 17. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(3), 493–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2007). Breaking down words to build meaning: Morphology, vocabulary, and reading comprehension in the urban classroom. The Reading Teacher, 61(2), 134–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2012). Knowledge of words, knowledge about words: Dimensions of vocabulary in first and second language learners in sixth grade. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 347–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kieffer, M. J., & Vukovic, R. K. (2012). Components and context: Exploring sources of reading difficulties for language minority learners and native English speakers in urban schools. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(5), 433–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Laufer, B. (1989). What percentage of text-lexis is essential for comprehension? In C. Lauren & M. Nordmann (Eds.), Special Language: From humans thinking to thinking machines (pp. 316–323). New Zealand, Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
- Laufer, B., & Goldstein, Z. (2004). Testing vocabulary knowledge: Size, strength, and computer adaptiveness. Language Learning, 54(3), 399–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lesaux, K., Crosson, A., Kieffer, M. J., & Pierce, M. (2010). Uneven profiles: Language minority learners’ word reading, vocabulary, and reading comprehension skills. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lesaux, K., & Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Exploring sources of reading comprehension difficulties among language minority learners and their classmates in early adolescence. American Educational Research Journal, 47(3), 596–632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lesaux, K., Koda, K., Siegel, L., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Development of literacy. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 75–149). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Luk, G., Mesite, L., Leon Guerrero, S., & Christodoulou, J. (2015). Reading outcomes in children with diverse language backgrounds. In Present at the International Bilingualism Symposium 10, May 20–24, New Brunswick, NJ.Google Scholar
- Marx, A. E., & Stanat, P. (2011). Reading comprehention of immigrant students in Germany: research evidence on determinats and target points for intervention. Reading and Writing, 25(8), 1929–1945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McKeown, M. G. (1985). The acquisition of word meaning from context by children of high and low ability. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 482–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McKeown, M. G. (2014). The nature of vocabulary acquisition. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
- Melby-Lervåg, M., & Lervåg, A. (2014). Reading comprehension and its underlying components in second-language learners: A meta-analysis of studies comparing first-and second-language learners. Psychological Bulletin, 140(2), 409–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 233–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nassaji, H. (2006). The relationship between depth of vocabulary knowledge and L2 learners’ lexical inferencing strategy use and success. The Modern Language Journal, 90(3), 387–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nation, K. (2005). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
- Nation, I. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed for reading and listening? Canadian Modern Language Review, 63(1), 59–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oetting, J. B., Rice, M. L., & Swank, L. K. (1995). Quick incidental learning (QUIL) of words by school-age children with and without SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38(2), 434–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(2), 191–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Paribakht, T. S., & Wesche, M. (1999). Reading and “incidental” L2 vocabulary acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(2), 195–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pasquarella, A., Gottardo, A., & Grant, A. (2012). Comparing factors related to reading comprehension in adolescents who speak English as first (L1) or second (L2) language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(6), 475–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Perfetti, C. H. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Perfetti, C. H., Landi, N., & Oakhill, J. (2005). The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 227–247). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pressley, M., Disney, L., & Anderson, K. (2007). Landmark vocabulary instructional research and the vocabulary instructional research that makes sense now. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 205–232). New York, NY: Gilford Press.Google Scholar
- Prior, A., Goldina, A., Shany, M., Geva, E., & Katzir, T. (2014). Lexical inference in L2: Predictive roles of vocabulary knowledge and reading skill beyond reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 27(8), 1467–1484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pulido, D. (2007). The relationship between text comprehension and second language incidental vocabulary acquisition: A matter of topic familiarity? Language Learning, 57(1), 155–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension. Report for the Office of Education Research and Improvement (OERI). Santa Monica, CA: RAND.Google Scholar
- Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V., Pimperton, H., & Nation, K. (2011). The role of self-teaching in learning orthographic and semantic aspects of new words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 47–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schmitt, N., Jiang, X., & Grabe, W. (2011). The percentage of words known in a text and reading comprehension. The Modern Language Journal, 95(1), 26–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schwartz, M., & Katzir, T. (2011). Depth of lexical knowledge among bilingual children: The impact of schooling. Reading and Writing, 25(8), 1947–1971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schwartz, M., Kozminsky, E., & Leikin, M. (2009). Delayed acquisition of irregular inflectional morphology in Hebrew in early sequential bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(4), 501–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shahar-Yames, D., & Prior, A. (2017). Reading development in upper elementary language minority readers of Hebrew: The specific challenge of fluency. Reading and Writing, 30(5), 1065–1087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shany, M., Lachman, D., Shalem, Z., Bahat, A., & Zeiger, T. (2006). Aleph-Taph: An assessment system of reading and writing disabilities. Tel Aviv: Yesod Publishing.Google Scholar
- Shefelbine, J. L. (1990). Student factors related to variability in learning word meanings from context. Journal of Literacy Research, 22(1), 71–97.Google Scholar
- Shiotsu, T., & Weir, C. J. (2007). The relative significance of syntactic knowledge and vocabulary breadth in the prediction of reading comprehension test performance. Language Testing, 24(1), 99–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Solberg, S., & Nevo, B. (1979). Initial steps towards an Israeli standardization of the Peabody Picture: Vocabulary Test PPVT. Megamot, 24(3), 407–413.Google Scholar
- Spencer, M., & Wagner, R. K. (2017). The comprehension problems for second-language learners with poor reading comprehension despite adequate decoding: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(2), 199–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89–115). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
- Swanborn, M., & De Glopper, K. (2002). Impact of reading purpose on incidental word learning from context. Language Learning, 52(1), 95–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Takacs, Z. K., Swart, E. K., & Bus, A. G. (2015). Benefits and pitfalls of multimedia and interactive features in technology-enhanced storybooks: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 698–739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Walters, J., Armon-Lotem, S., Altman, C., Topaj, N., & Gagarina, N. (2014). Language proficiency and social identity in Russian-Hebrew and Russian-German preschool children. In R. K. Silbereisen, F. Titzmann, & Y. Shavit (Eds.), The challenges of diaspora migration: Interdisciplinary perspectives on Israel and Germany (pp. 45–62). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
- Webb, S. (2010). A corpus driven study of the potential for vocabulary learning through watching movies. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(4), 497–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wesche, M. B., & Paribakht, T. (2009). Lexical inferencing in a first and second language: Cross-linguistic dimensions. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
- Zeltsman-Kulick, R., Katzir, T., & Prior, A. (2018) English reading comprehension of adolescent Hebrew speakers learning English as foreign language. (in progress).Google Scholar