Skip to main content
Log in

Rapid serial naming and reading ability: the role of lexical access

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rapid serial naming tasks are frequently used to explain variance in reading skill. However, the construct being measured by rapid naming is yet undetermined. The Phonological Processing theory suggests that rapid naming relates to reading because of similar demands of access to long-term stored phonological representations of visual stimuli. Some researchers have argued that isolated or discrete-trial naming is a more precise measure of lexical access than serial naming, thus it is likely that any shared variance between these two formats can be attributed to similar lexical access demands. The present study examined whether there remained any variance in reading ability that could be uniquely explained by the rapid naming task while controlling for isolated naming. Structural equation modeling was used to examine these relations within the context of the phonological processing model. Results indicated that serial naming uniquely predicted reading, and the relation was stronger with isolated naming controlled for, suggesting that isolated naming functioned as a suppressor variable in the relation of serial naming with reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2005). Early literacy from a longitudinal perspective. Educational Research and Evaluation, 11, 253–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). Amos (Version 5.0) [computer software]. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. (1989). Naming speed and phonological awareness: Independent contributors to reading disabilities. National Reading Conference Yearbook, 38, 165–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing, 7, 189–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 5, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 400–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in ran and reading: A response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eakin, S., & Douglas, V. (1971). “Automatization” and oral reading problems in children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 26–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2005). Impaired visual processing of multielement arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research, 45, 855–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. W., Obregon, M., Kelly, M. L., & Pranigan, H. P. (2008). Elucidating the component processes involved in dyslexic and non-dyslexic reading fluency: Evidence from eye-tracking. Cognition, 109, 389–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1996). Models of speech perception and phonological processing in reading. Child Development, 67, 1836–1856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 11, 172–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, K., Fussenegger, Bl., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing: Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatized naming (ran) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Reaction times and dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 14, 707–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. C. Erhi, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Finger, E., & Hogaboam, T. W. (1978). Sources of vocalization latency differences between skilled and less-skilled young readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 730–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M. (2003). The role of naming speed, phonological processing and morphological/syntactic skill in the reading and spelling performance of second-grade children. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition. Special Issue on Language Disorders and Reading Acquisition, 10(1), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H., & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 46–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, J., & Everatt, J. (2005). Reception class predictors of literacy skills. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 171–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C., & Davis, J. M. (1988). Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staller, J., & Sekuler, R. (1975). Children read normal and reversed letters: A simple test of reading skill. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 539–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1981). Relationships between word decoding speed, general name-retrieval ability, and reading progress in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 809–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Feeman, D. J., & Cunningham, A. E. (1983). The development of the relation between letter-naming speed and reading ability. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 199–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1982). Automatization failure in learning disabilities. Topics in Learning Disabilities, 2, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. Review of Educational Research, 73, 407–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhry, J. K. (2002). Basic functions of language, reading and reading disability. Neuropsychology and cognition: vol. 20. Name of volume (pp. 299–313). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vukovic, R. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). The double-deficit hypothesis: A comprehensive analysis of the evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 25–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. (1993). Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, D. J., Price, G. G., & Gillingham, M. G. (1988). The critical but transitory importance of letter naming. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 108–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 415–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery test—revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

One of the authors (J. A. R. Logan) received pre-doctoral training support during the conduct of this research through grant R305B04074 from the Institute of Education Sciences to Florida State University. Support for writing this article has been provided by Grant P50 HD052120 from NICHD.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica A. R. Logan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Logan, J.A.R., Schatschneider, C. & Wagner, R.K. Rapid serial naming and reading ability: the role of lexical access. Read Writ 24, 1–25 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9199-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9199-1

Keywords

Navigation