Skip to main content
Log in

Prefix identification in the reading of Dutch bisyllabic words

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted in order to explore the role of prefix identification in the reading of Dutch bisyllabic words. Although Dutch orthography is highly regular, several deviations from a one-to-one correspondence exist. A case in point is the grapheme E which can represent the vowels ε, e and œ in polysyllabic words. In Experiment 1, 33 third-grade children and 46 sixth-grade children were presented a list of randomly ordered bisyllabic words starting with the letter string BE and the first syllable being (1) a real prefix, (2) a phonological prefix (same sound pattern as a prefix), or (3) a pseudoprefix (sound pattern deviant from a prefix). Pseudowords starting with the same letter string were also presented. The results showed that words starting with a real or a phonological prefix were identified more accurately than words starting with a pseudoprefix. For the pseudowords, a predominant interpretation of the first part as a prefix was also evidenced. In Experiment 2, a lexical decision task was administered to 35 third-grade children, 33 sixth-grade children, and 26 adults. Words with a phonological prefix and words with a pseudoprefix were randomly presented along with other word types. The data showed both children and adults to retrieve words with phonological prefixes more quickly and more accurately than words with a pseudoprefix. The results are discussed with reference to current models of word decoding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Assink E. M. H., Vooijs C. and Knuijt P. P. N. A. (2000). Prefixes as access units in visual word recognition: A comparison of Italian and Dutch data. Reading and Writing 12: 149–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen R. H. and Piepenbrock R. (1993). The CELEX lexical data base on CD-ROM. Linguistic DataConsortium, Philadelphia, PA

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown P., Lupker S. J. and Colombo L. (1994). Interacting sources of information in word naming: A study of individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 537–554

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In: (eds) Morphological aspects of language processing, pp 189–209. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing 12: 169–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle J. F. and Nomanbhoy D. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics 14: 177–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombo L. (1992). Lexical stress effect and its interaction with frequency in word pronunciation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18: 987–1003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M., Rastle K., Perry C., Langdon R. and Ziegler J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108: 204–256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Distributed representations. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Vol. 1: Foundations (pp. 77–109). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

  • Kooij J. G. (1994). Dutch. In: Comrie, B. (eds) The major languages of Western Europe, pp 139–156. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudanna A. and Burani C. (1995). Distributional properties of derivational affixes: Implications for processing. In: Feldman, L. B. (eds) Morphological aspects of language processing, pp 345–364. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong C. K. (2000). Rapid processing of base and derived forms of words and grades 4, 5 and 6 children’s spelling. Reading and Writing 12: 169–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt W. J. M. (1999). Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 223–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy W., Diakidoy I. and Anderson R. (1993). The acquisition of morphology: Learning the contribution of suffixes to the meaning of derivates. Journal of reading Behavior 25: 155–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In: Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C. and Treiman, R. (eds) Reading acquisition, pp 145–174. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut D. C. and Gonnerman L. M. (2000). Are nonsemantic morphological effects incompatible with a distributed connectionist approach to lexical processing?. Language and Cognitive Processing 15: 445–485

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut D. C., McClelland J. L., Seidenberg M. S. and Patterson K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103: 56–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle K. and Coltheart M. (2000). Lexical and nonlexical print-to-sound translation of disyllabic words and nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language 42: 342–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma P. and Verhoeven L. (1990). Acquisition of reading in Dutch. Foris, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaerlaekens A., Kohnstamm D. and Lejaegere M. (1999). Streeflijst woordenschat voor zesjarigen. Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder R. and Baayen R. H. (1994). Prefix stripping revisited. Journal of Memory and Language 33: 357–375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder R. and Baayen R. H. (1995). Modeling morphological processing. In: (eds) Morphological aspects of language processing, pp 131–157. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg M. S. and Gonnerman L. M. (2000). Explaining derivational morphology as the convergence of codes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 353–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staphorsius G. and Krom R. (1988). Frequenties van woordvormen en letterposities in jeugdliteratuur. Cito, Arnhem

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgeson J. T. (2001). The theory and practice of intervention: comparing outcomes from prevention and remediation studies. In: Fawcett, A. (eds) Dyslexia, theory and good practice, pp 203–217. Whurr, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldinger S. D. (1994). Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 1269–1291

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound and reading. Memory & Cognition 15: 181–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldinger S. D. (1996). Phonological mediation in skilled and dyslexic reading. In: Chase, C. H., Rosen, G. D. and Sherman, G. F. (eds) Developmental dyslexia: Neural, cognitive and genetic mechanisms, pp 185–223. York Press, Timonium, MD

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The role of morphology in learning to read. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 209–271

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, H. (2003). Units of analysis in reading Dutch bisyllabic pseudowords. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 255–271

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ludo Verhoeven.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Verhoeven, L., Schreuder, R. & Haarman, V. Prefix identification in the reading of Dutch bisyllabic words. Read Writ 19, 651–668 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-1912-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-1912-0

Keywords

Navigation