Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Mechanisms of Change in a Cognitive Behavioral Couples Prevention Program: Does Being Naughty or Nice Matter?

  • Published:
Prevention Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although there is a body of evidence suggesting beneficial effects of premarital prevention, little research directly examines the mechanisms of effect. One study that examined changes in communication following training in the Prevention and Relationship Enhancement Program (PREP) found that, although couples made the expected communication gains pre to post PREP, female gains in positive communication were paradoxically associated with worse, not better, outcomes (Schilling et al., J. Fam. Psychol. 17(1):41–53, 2003). Using two samples, the current investigation did not yield evidence of such an association. We discuss issues related to replication studies (e.g., failure to reject null hypotheses), challenges in analyzing and interpreting dyadic data, and implications for prevention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We decided that missing data could not be imputed for couples who were excluded because they did not complete PREP or did not marry; these couples were excluded for conceptual reasons. These couples differ in meaningful ways from the sample we needed to focus on in the current investigation. Regarding those couples excluded because they did not complete POST and/or a later follow up, we elected not to impute their data because it is difficult to know for sure that they are missing at random and because the use of cut-off scores and changes scores calls for precision that imputed values would not allow. We were concerned that imputation would introduce too much noise into the analyses. Such unreliability seems to be a particularly salient problem in a study focused on replication because it would make finding significant results harder (i.e., statistical power would decrease).

  2. To classify participants as distressed or not, Schilling et al. calculated reliable change in relationship adjustment from POST scores on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. It seems more logical to assess changes from PRE, given the interest in knowing if couples improve or at least maintain levels of adjustment that were present prior to preventive intervention. We ran analyses both ways, using change in relationship adjustment over time from both PRE levels and POST levels. There were few differences. In an effort to be consistent with Schilling et al. (2003), we present results that are directly testing the same questions they addressed with the variables entered simultaneously (as done by Schilling et al.) based on change from POST scores. For analyses using one gender’s scores at a time (those we feel are both most parsimonious and most straightforward), we calculated distress based on declines from PRE (20 of 104 females and 20 of 103 males) which seemed more directly related to the assessment of prevention effects.

  3. Schilling et al. explain in detail their use of a dichotomous outcome variable in assessing the impact of PRE to POST communication changes on marital outcomes. Participants’ outcomes were categorized as either non-distressed or distressed. Distressed was defined as either becoming divorced or demonstrating a reliable decline in relationship adjustment scores, into the distressed range of scores on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; a measure highly similar to the MAT). Schilling et al. calculated the reliable change index as suggested by Jacobson and Truax (1991). Using the estimate of .96 for the reliability of the DAS, participants with a negative change of at least 7.7 at any follow-up point up to 5 years post intervention (compared to POST scores) and a DAS score <104 were classified as distressed (or if they became divorced). Because of a lower level of reliability for the MAT in our samples, we obtained a criterion for reliable change of 22.5. To be sure that we were not categorizing individuals as non-distressed that the method of Schilling et al. would have classified as distressed, we examined the logistic regression analyses for the FSP sample described below using both our criterion of 22.4 and their criterion of 7.7. The results were indistinguishable. All but two individuals were in the same category either way.

  4. Survival analysis involves equations in which time is modeled in order to examine the timing and odds of onset. Typically, separate variables are used to indicate distinct assessment points. Schilling et al. (2003) modified survival analysis (i.e., using one variable to represent time rather than separate variables) that we were unable to find precedent for in the literature. This may, however, be of little consequence since timing was not a focus of their analyses or ours.

  5. In addition to this alteration, we decided not to include the PRE interaction scores in these regressions. Schilling et al. included the PRE positive communication scores in addition to the residual scores. However, it seems redundant to enter both the PRE score and the residual score, because, as Lord (1967) indicates, entering either the PRE score and the POST score together or the residual score would essentially yield the same result. With the residual score, the PRE score is already being controlled for. We ran analyses both ways, and the results were essentially the same. Therefore, including both variables seems to do little more than unnecessarily reduce power by the effect on the degrees of freedom (also see Kenny 1998; also see Wainer 1991).

  6. As noted earlier, we favor doing these analyses looking at change from PRE, rather than POST. However, we also examined the results with change in distress from POST, to be consistent with Table 1. The models were non-significant, and the odds ratios indicated that there was no hint of changes in positive communication predicting distress development (odds ratios ranged from 1.03 to 1.18).

References

  • Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62(4), 1269–1287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkins, D. C. (2005). Using multilevel models to analyze couple and family treatment data: Basic and advanced issues. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(1), 98–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baucom, D. H., Epstein, N. B., Burnett, C. K., & Rankin, L. A. (1993). Conflict in marriage: A cognitive/behavioral formulation. In S. Worchel & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Conflict between people and groups: Causes, processes, and resolutions. (pp. 7–29). Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baucom, D. H., Hahlweg, K., Atkins, D. C., Engl, J., & Thurmaier, F. (2006). Long-term prediction of marital quality following a relationship education program: Being positive in a constructive way. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 448–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. S., & Doherty, W. J. (2003). Evaluating the effectiveness of premarital prevention programs: A meta-analytic review of outcome research. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 52(2), 105–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, M. L., Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J. (2004). Before they said “I do”: Discriminating among marital outcomes over 13 years based on premarital data. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 66, 613–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coie, J. D., Watt, N. F., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., Asarnow, J. R., Markman, H. J., et al. (1993). The science of prevention: A conceptual framework and some directions for a national research program. American Psychologist, 48(10), 1013–1022.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crane, D. R., Allgood, S. M., Larson, J. H., & Griffin, W. (1990). Assessing marital quality with distressed and nondistressed couples: A comparison and equivalency table for three frequently used measures. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52(1), 87–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doss, B. D., Thum, Y. M., Sevier, M., Atkins, D. C., & Christensen, A. (2005). Improving relationships: Mechanisms of change in couple therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(4), 624–633.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Draper, N., & Smith, H. (1981). Applied regression analysis (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincham, F. D., Stanley, S. M., & Beach, S. R. H. (2007). Transformative processes in marriage: An analysis of emerging trends. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 69(2), 275–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1993). The roles of conflict engagement, escalation, and avoidance in marital interaction: A longitudinal view of five types of couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(1), 6–15.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M., & Krokoff, L. J. (1989). Marital interaction and satisfaction: A longitudinal view. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57(1), 47–52.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hahlweg, K., Markman, H. J., Thurmaier, F., Engl, J., & Eckert, V. (1998). Prevention of marital distress: Results of a German prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 12(4), 543–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halford, W. K., Markman, H. J., Kline, G. H., & Stanley, S. M. (2003). Best practice in couple relationship education. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 29(3), 385–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halford, W. K., Sanders, M. R., & Behrens, B. C. (2001). Can skills training prevent relationship problems in at-risk couples? Four-year effects of a behavioral relationship education program. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(4), 750–768.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heavey, C. L., Layne, C., & Christensen, A. (1993). Gender and conflict structure in marital interaction: A replication and extension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(1), 16–27.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huston, T. L., Caughlin, J. P., Houts, R. M., Smith, S. E., & George, L. J. (2001). The connubial crucible: Newlywed years as predictors of marital delight, distress, and divorce. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 237–252.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(1), 12–19.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Julien, D., Markman, H. J., & Lindahl, K. M. (1989). A comparison of a global and a microanalytic coding system: Implications for future trends in studying interactions. Behavioral Assessment, 11, 81–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3–34.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the trajectory of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(5), 1075–1092.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Keiley, M. K., & Martin, N. C. (2005). Survival analysis in family research. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(1), 142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A. (1998). Couples, gender, and time: Comments on method. In T. N. Bradbury (Ed.), Developmental course of marital dysfunction (pp. 410–422). Cambridge University Press.

  • Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D., & Cook, W. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, G. H., Julien, D., Baucom, B., Hartman, S. G., Gilbert, K., Gonzales, T., et al. (2004). The interactional dimensions coding system: A global system for couple interactions. In P. K. Kerig & D. H. Baucom (Eds.), Couple observational coding systems (pp. 113–127). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, F. M. (1967). A paradox in the interpretation of group comparisons. Psychological Bulletin, 68, 304–305.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J. (1981). Prediction of marital distress: A 5-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49(5), 760–762.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J., & Floyd, F. (1980). Possibilities for the prevention of marital discord: A behavioral perspective. American Journal of Family Therapy, 8(2), 29–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J., Floyd, F. J., Stanley, S. M., & Jamieson, K. (1984). A cognitive-behavioral program for the prevention of marital and family distress: Issues in program development and delivery. In K. Halweg & N. S. Jacobson (Eds.), Marital interaction. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J., Floyd, F. J., Stanley, S. M., & Storaasli, R. D. (1988). Prevention of marital distress: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(2), 210–217.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J., & Hahlweg, K. (1993). The prediction and prevention of marital distress: An international perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(1), 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J., Renick, M. J., Floyd, F. J., Stanley, S. M., & Clements, M. (1993). Preventing marital distress through communication and conflict management training: A four and five year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 70–77.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, H. J., Stanley, S. M., Blumberg, S. L., Jenkins, N., & Whiteley, C. (2004). Twelve hours to a great marriage. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendenhall, W., & Sincich, T. (2003). A second course in statistics: Regression analysis (6th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, D., & Peck, E. (1982). Introduction to linear regression analysis. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notarius, C. I., & Markman, H. J. (1993). We can work it out: Making sense of marital conflict. New York, NY: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notarius, C. I., & Vanzetti, N. (1983). Marital agendas protocol. In E. E. Filsinger (Ed.), Marriage and family assessment: A sourcebook for family therapy (pp. 209–227). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parke, M., & Ooms, T. (2002). More than a dating service? State activities designed to strengthen and promote marriage (No. 2, Clasp policy brief: Couples and marriage series). Washington D.C.: Center for Law and Social Policy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prado, L. M., & Markman, H. J. (1998). For better or worse the second time around: Analyzing the communication patterns of remarried couples. In M. Cox & J. Brookes-Gunn (Eds.), The formation, functioning and stability of families. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichardt, C., & Gollob, H. (1986). Satisfying the constraints of causal modeling. In W. K. Trochim (Ed.), Advances in quasi-experimental design and analysis. New directions for program evaluation (vol. 31). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogge, R. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Till violence does us part: The differing roles of communication and aggression in predicting adverse marital outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(3), 340–351.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sayers, S. L., Kohn, C. S., & Heavey, C. (1998). Prevention of marital dysfunction: Behavioral approaches and beyond. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(6), 713–744.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schilling, E. A., Baucom, D. H., Burnett, C. K., Allen, E. S., & Ragland, L. (2003). Altering the course of marriage: The effect of PREP communication skills acquisition on couples’ risk of becoming maritally distressed. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(1), 41–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, S. M. (2001). Making a case for premarital education. Family Relations, 50, 272–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, S. M., Amato, P. R., Johnson, C. A., & Markman, H. J. (2006). Premarital education, marital quality, and marital stability: Findings from a large, random, household survey. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 117–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, S. M., Blumberg, S. L., & Markman, H. J. (1999). Helping couples fight for their marriages: The PREP approach. In R. Berger & M. T. Hannah (Eds.), Preventive approaches in couples therapy (pp. 279–303). Philadelphia, PA, US: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, S. M., Markman, H. J., Prado, L. M., Olmos-Gallo, P. A., Tonelli, L., St. Peters, M., et al. (2001). Community-based premarital prevention: Clergy and lay leaders on the front lines. Family Relations, 50(1), 67–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, S. M., Markman, H. J., & Whitton, S. W. (2002). Communication, conflict and commitment: Insights on the foundations of relationship success from a national survey. Family Process, 41(4), 659–675.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, K. T., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Are premarital prevention programs reaching couples at risk for marital dysfunction? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(1), 24–30.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wainer, H. (1991). Adjusting for differential base rates: Lord’s paradox again. Psychological Bulletin, 109(1), 147–151.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Willett, J. B., & Singer, J. D. (1993). Investigating onset, cessation, relapse, and recovery: Why you should, and how you can, use discrete-time survival analysis to examine event occurrence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(6), 952–965.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Woody, E. Z., & Costanzo, P. R. (1990). Does marital agony precede marital ecstasy? A comment on Gottman and Krokoff’s “Marital interaction and satisfaction: A longitudinal view.” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58(4), 499–501.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We thank the anonymous reviewers for their excellent questions and comments. This research was supported by two grants from The National Institutes of Health: 1RO1HD48780-1A1 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and 5-RO1-MH35525-12 from The National Institute of Mental Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott M. Stanley.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stanley, S.M., Rhoades, G.K., Olmos-Gallo, P.A. et al. Mechanisms of Change in a Cognitive Behavioral Couples Prevention Program: Does Being Naughty or Nice Matter?. Prev Sci 8, 227–239 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-007-0071-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-007-0071-8

Keywords

Navigation