Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Threat Perception and American Support for Torture

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When do Americans support the government’s use of torture? We argue that perceptions of threat undermine the extent to which American public opinion serves as a bulwark against government torture. Although surveys demonstrate that a slim majority of the American public generally opposes torture, using a nationally-representative survey experiment, we show that Americans are considerably more supportive of government abuse when it is directed at individuals who they perceive as threatening: specifically, when a detainee has an Arabic name and when the alleged crime is terrorism. Given the malleability of public opinion as a potential constraint on abuse, our results underscore the importance of institutional protections of human rights.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For public opinion to influence government policy, leaders also must acquiesce to the will of the people. We address this requirement in additional detail below.

  2. Torture is illegal under domestic and international law in the United States. The Eighth Amendment to the United States Constitution [e.g., Wilkerson v. Utah, 99 U.S. 130 (1878)] and the 1996 U.S. War Crimes Act prohibit the United States government from engaging in torture. The U.S. has further condemned torture by signing and ratifying the 1949 Geneva Conventions, the 1966 International Convention on Civil and Political Rights, and the 1984 Convention Against Torture and Other Cruel, Inhuman or Degrading Treatment or Punishment (CAT).

  3. In May 2014, Amnesty International published results from public opinion surveys in twenty-one countries showing that support for government abuse of people detained for threats to public safety varies from a low of 12% (Greece) to as high as 74% (China and India), with 36% of respondents worldwide supporting the use of abusive tactics (Amnesty International 2014). Citizens in authoritarian regimes may view human rights violations like torture differently than citizens in democracies (Blakeley 2007). In this article, we focus solely on public opinion in the United States.

  4. A few studies examine aggregate-level differences across countries. In cross-national comparisons, per capita income (Miller 2011), general respect for rights (Miller 2011), and political development (Mayer et al. 2014) have been found to be negatively correlated with support for torture.

  5. These levels of support are a radical departure from the past. During World War II, support for torture among Americans was in the single digits, except when the hypothetical detainee was Adolf Hitler, in which case it rose to 20%. Between World War II and 9/11, surveys asking Americans about support of U.S. government torture do not exist (Miller et al. 2014, pp. 18–21).

  6. While dismissing the notion that these techniques are “torture,” former Vice President Richard Cheney has repeatedly defended the use of “enhanced interrogation techniques” since leaving office. “I would do it again in a minute”...“Torture is what the Al Qaeda terrorists did to 3000 Americans on 9/11. There is no comparison between that and what we did with respect to enhanced interrogation” (Shane 2014). The Vice President’s position was representative of many in the Bush Administration, although some argued against the legal standing of enhanced interrogation techniques. See Goldsmith (2007) for an insider’s account of the debate about torture within the Bush Administration.

  7. For example, Hendrix and Wong (2013, p. 58) argue, “Colombians voted for Uribe precisely because they knew he would be willing to violate international human rights standards in pursuit of ending the civil war.”

  8. For public opinion to serve as an actual constraint against torture in democracies, governments (i.e., executive agents) must be responsive to public policy demands. Relative to other policies, the executive has a great deal of control over decisions to engage in—or limit—the use of torture. Public opinion, even when amounting to a clear consensus, is only operative if it is reflected in the actions of executive behavior regarding torture. Additionally, in cases where opinion is divided, government leaders may not consider the voices of all citizens equally. Indeed, recent work by Bartels (2009) and Gilens and Page (2014, p. 565) argues that public policy decisions in the U.S. call the responsiveness of governmental actors into question and suggest a bias in political influence, noting that “mass-based interest groups and average citizens have little or no independent influence” on government policy.

  9. One could argue that by supporting torture under conditions of threat, some individuals may hope to “protect the lives” of the community at large. In our view, such a claim simply illustrates the fickle (situational) nature of public support for human rights. Under perceptions of threat, citizens are willing to allow their government to violate one of the fundamental tenets of human rights and international law—the prohibition against torture.

  10. Many scholars argue that the influence of threat on political tolerance is conditional on individual-level characteristics including predispositions (i.e., personality) and political attitudes (Marcus 1995), ethnocentrism (Kam et al. 2007), authoritarianism (Hetherington and Suhay 2011), and political party affiliation (Malhotra and Popp 2012). Because our goal is to determine the effect of context on support for government torture, we employ an experimental design that effectively controls (via random assignment) for individual-level covariates.

  11. These stereotypes are often exacerbated by the media (Altheide 2006), which is less likely to engage in balanced reporting following threatening events (Nacos and Torres-Reyna 2007).

  12. These results echo those of Davis (2007), who finds that respondents of the 2001 National Civil Liberties Survey were less supportive of protecting the rights of non-citizens and foreigners than other individuals.

  13. See also Hogg and Abrams (1993), Brewer (1999, 2001), Duckitt and Mphuthing (1998), Hodson et al. (2003), Kinder and Sears (1981), Lahav (2004), Shamir and Sagiv-Schifter (2006), Sherif (1966), Sniderman and Hagendoorn (2007), Stephan and Stephan (2000, 1996), and Tajfel (1981).

  14. Our approach differs from that of Huddy et al. (2005) in several ways. Conceptually, we are interested in a broader conceptualization of “threat” than terrorism. Our conceptualization and operationalization of threat are discussed in additional detail in the next section of the paper. Empirically, we employ an experimental research design rather than simple observational analysis; random assignment of the treatments in our experiments allow us to investigate the effect of several types of threat on support for torture, independent of individual respondent characteristics.

  15. In contrast, Miller, Gronke and Rejali (2014, pp. 27–33) argue that when forming opinions about complex topics, Americans take their cues from political elites rather than their consumption of fiction.

  16. See also Miller, Gronke and Rejali (2014, pp. 16–17) and Carlsmith and Sood (2009).

  17. See also McCoy (2006).

  18. Gould and Klor (2010) use a similar method to show that the threat of terrorism makes Israelis more willing to grant territorial concessions to the Palestinians. Beyond a certain threshold, however, the threat of terrorism makes Israelis less likely to grant concessions to the Palestinians.

  19. Unlike the majority of the extant literature on public opinion and government torture, our approach is experimental in nature and consequently yields higher levels of internal validity.

  20. Further details regarding the CCES sample design can be found at http://projects.iq.harvard.edu/cces/book/sample-design. The response rate for the 2012 CCES was 44%. The data and codebook can be accessed at http://hdl.handle.net/1902.1/21447.

  21. In what follows, we present a 3 \(\times\) 2 experimental design. Our original CCES survey experiment employed a 4 \(\times\) 2 design. In addition to the the three Male conditions described below, we also included in our original survey a White Female condition. Respondents were thus randomly assigned to one of eight conditions: (1) Arab Male Allegedly Criminal, (2) Arab Male Allegedly Terrorist, (3) Latino Male Allegedly Criminal, (4) Latino Male Allegedly Terrorist, (5) White Male Allegedly Criminal, (6) White Male Allegedly Terrorist, (7) White Female Allegedly Criminal, (8) White Female Allegedly Terrorist. Rather than present the results of the original 4 \(\times\) 2 design below, we present results for a 3 \(\times\) 2 design because respondents who received the Female treatment were no more or less likely to support abusive treatment than those in the White Male treatments, we drop respondents who received the Female treatment from the analyses reported below to focus on the role of race/ethnicity and situational threat.

  22. Davenport et al. (2011) similarly argue that because protest events with African-American participants are deemed more “threatening,” such events will be met with increased police presence and violence.

  23. Elected politicians recognize that labeling a dissident (group) as a “terrorist(s)” delegitimizes the dissident (group) (Truman 2003). As such, Tilly (2004) argues that the terms, “terror, terrorism, and terrorist do not identify causally coherent and distinct social phenomena but strategies that recur across a wide variety of actors and political situations.”

  24. An additional implication of our argument suggests—perhaps counterintuitively—that democratic countries should be more likely to increase their use of torture in response to terror tactics than non-democratic ones. Conrad et al. (2016) show that while the amount of torture countries are accused of committing is not correlated with the number of terror attacks that occur in the country, the amount of torture that military personnel are accused of committing is positively associated with the number of transnational terror attacks that occur in that country. Further, the relationship is stronger in democracies than in autocratic countries.

  25. We chose to present a “crime” scenario that insinuates the detainee is undocumented. We assume that respondents see lack of documentation as a crime. To the extent that this assumption does not hold, we are less likely to find support for our main hypotheses.

  26. The reader may be concerned that the reference to the Oklahoma City bombing, an incident from over two decades ago, may produce a heterogeneous treatment effect, where older or better educated respondents pick up on the terror cue, but younger or less educated respondents, who are less familiar with the event, do not. We do not find heterogeneous treatment effects in our analysis of the Terror versus Non-Terror conditions that follow. See Model 4 in Table B3 of our Online Appendix for details.

  27. Although we speak about increased levels of support for torture, it is proper to note that only values greater than 4 indicate agreement with torture.

  28. Please see our Online Appendix for a number of alternative model specifications that demonstrate the robustness of our results. All of those results support the same inferences regarding Hypotheses 1 and 2.

  29. It is possible that our reported Arab treatment effect occurs because respondents make assumptions of terrorism when an Arab name is mentioned and that our Latino treatment effect occurs because respondents make assumptions of crime when a Latino name is mentioned.

  30. Please refer to the Online Appendix for additional details.

  31. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.

  32. Although elections are often argued to provide leaders with incentives to better respect human rights (Richards 1999; Cingranelli and Filippov 2010), recent work suggests that elections do not protect against government abuse of the right to freedom from torture (e.g., Ron 1997; Rejali 2007; Fair et al. 2010).

References

  • Altheide, D. L. (2006). The mass media, crime and terrorism. Journal of International Criminal Justice, 4(5), 982–997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amnesty International. (2014). Stop torture global survey: Attitudes to torture. London. http://www.amnesty.org/en/library/info/ACT40/005/2014/en.

  • Anderson, C. J., Paskeviciute, A., Sandovici, M. E., & Tverdova, Y. V. (2005). In the eye of the beholder? The foundations of subjective human rights conditions in East-Central Europe. Comparative Political Studies, 38(7), 771–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., Regan, P. M., & Ostergard, R. L. (2002). Political repression and public perceptions of human rights. Political Research Quarterly, 55(2), 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, K. T., & Seguin, C. (2015). Group threat and policy change: The spatial dynamics of prohibition politics, 1890–1919. American Journal of Sociology, 121(2), 475–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (2009). Unequal democracy: The political economy of the new gilded age. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berrebi, C., & Klor, E. F. (2008). Are voters sensitive to terrorism? Direct evidence from the Israeli electorate. American Political Science Review, 102(03), 279–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review, 94(4), 991–1013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakeley, R. (2007). Why torture? Review of International Studies, 33(03), 373–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobo, L. D. (1999). Prejudice as group position: Microfoundations of a sociological approach to racism and race relations. Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 445–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. (2005). Understanding interaction models: Improving empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 14(1), 63–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branton, R., Cassese, E. C., Jones, B. S., & Westerland, C. (2011). All along the watchtower: Acculturation fear, anti-Latino affect, and immigration. Journal of Politics, 73(3), 664–679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love and outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B. (2001). The many faces of social identity: Implications for political psychology. Political Psychology, 22(1), 115–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, D. M., & Broockman, D. E. (2011). Do politicians racially discriminate against constituents? A field experiment on state legislators. American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 463–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, K. M., & Sood, A. M. (2009). The fine line between interrogation and retribution. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 191–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, M., & Listhaug, O. (2007). Citizens’ perceptions of human rights practices: An analysis of 55 countries. Journal of Peace Research, 44(4), 465–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpusor, A. G., & Loges, W. E. (2006). Rental discrimination and ethnicity in names. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36(4), 934–952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cingranelli, D. L., & Filippov, M. (2010). Electoral rules and incentives to protect human rights. Journal of Politics, 72(1), 243–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, C. R., Conrad, J., Walsh, J. I., & Piazza, J. A. (2016). Who tortures the terrorists? Transnational terrorism and military torture. Foreign Policy Analysis. doi:10.1111/fpa.12066.

  • Conrad, C. R., & Moore, W. H. (2010). What stops the torture? American Journal of Political Science, 54(2), 459–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, C. (1996). Constitutional promises’ and repressive reality: A cross-national time-series investigation of why political and civil liberties are suppressed. Journal of Politics, 58(3), 627–654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, C. (2007). State repression and the domestic democratic peace. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, C., Soule, S. A., & Armstrong, D. A. (2011). Protesting while black? The differential policing of American activism, 1960–1990. American Sociological Review, 76(1), 152–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, D. W. (2007). Negative liberty: Public opinion and the terrorist attacks on America. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donnelly, J. (2003). Universal human rights in theory and practice (2nd ed.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downes, A. B. (2011). Targeting civilians in war. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake, B. (2014). Americans’ views on use of torture in fighting terrorism have been mixed. Washington, DC: Pew Fact Tank.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duckitt, J., & Mphuthing, T. (1998). Group identification and intergroup attitudes: A longitudinal analysis in South Africa. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 80–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhardt, J. L., Davies, P. G., Purdie-Vaughns, V. J., & Johnson, S. L. (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of Black defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 17(5), 383–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fair, C. C., Malhotra, N., & Shapiro, J. N. (2010). Islam, militancy, and politics in Pakistan: Insights from a national sample. Terrorism and Political Violence, 22(4), 495–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farley, J. E. (1987). Disproportionate Black and Hispanic unemployment in US metropolitan areas: The Roles of racial inequality, segregation and discrimination in male joblessness. American Journal of Economics and Sociology, 46(2), 129–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Harris, L. T., & Cuddy, A. J. C. (2004). Why ordinary people torture enemy prisoners. Science, 306(5701), 1482–1483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galanter, M., & Luban, D. (1992). Poetic justice: Punitive damages and legal pluralism. American University Law Review, 42, 1393–1463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerges, F. A. (2003). Islam and Muslims in the mind of America. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 588(1), 73–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getmansky, A., & Zeitzoff, T. (2014). Terrorism and voting: The effect of rocket threat on voting in Israeli elections. American Political Science Review, 108(03), 588–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. L. (2008). Intolerance and political repression in the United States: A half century after McCarthyism. American Journal of Political Science, 52(1), 96–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilens, M., & Page, B. I. (2014). Testing theories of American politics: Elites, interest groups, and average citizens. Perspectives on Politics, 12(03), 564–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M. J., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, J. (2007). The terror presidency: Law and judgment inside the Bush administration. NewYork: WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, E. D., & Klor, E. F. (2010). Does terrorism work? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 125(4), 1459–1510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gronke, P., Rejali, D., Drenguis, D., Hicks, J., Miller, P., & Nakayama, B. (2010). U.S. public opinion on torture, 2001–2009. PS. Political Science and Politics, 43(3), 437–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gronke, P., Rejali, D. & Miller, P. (2014). No, Americans aren’t ‘Fine with Torture.’ They strongly reject it. Washington, DC. Retrieved December 11, 2014 from http://www.washingtonpost.com/posteverything/wp/2014/12/11/no-americans-arent-fine-with-torture-they-strongly-reject-it/.

  • Haider-Markel, D. P., & Vieux, A. (2008). Gender and conditional support for torture in the war on terror. Politics & Gender, 4(1), 5–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., & Hiscox, M. J. (2010). Attitudes toward highly skilled and low-skilled immigration: Evidence from a survey experiment. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 61–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., & Hopkins, D. J. (2015). The hidden American immigration consensus: A conjoint analysis of attitudes toward immigrants. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 529–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). Dehumanized perception: A psychological means to facilitate atrocities, torture, and genocide? Journal of Psychology, 219(3), 175–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrix, C. S., & Wong, W. H. (2013). When is the pen truly mighty? Regime type and the efficacy of naming and shaming in curbing human rights abuses. British Journal of Political Science, 43(3), 651–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M., & Suhay, E. (2011). Authoritarianism, threat, and Americans’ support for the war on terror. American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 546–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodson, G., Dovidio, J. F., & Esses, V. M. (2003). Ingroup identification as a moderator of positive-negative asymmetry in social discrimination. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(2), 215–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1993). Towards a single-process uncertainty-reduction model of social motivation in groups. In A. Michael (Ed.), Group motivation: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 173–190). Hertfordshire: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooks, G., & Mosher, C. (2005). Outrages against personal dignity: Rationalizing abuse and torture in the war on terror. Social Forces, 83(4), 1627–1645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, J. D., & Bingham Powell, G. (1994). Congruence between citizens and policymakers in two visions of liberal democracy. World Politics, 46(3), 291–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 593–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. D. (1992). Anti-Arabic prejudice in ‘Middletown.’ Psychological Reports, 70(3), 811–818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, P. W. (2008). Sacred violence: Torture, terror, and sovereignty. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C. D., & Kinder, D. R. (2007). Terror and ethnocentrism: Foundations of American support for the war on terrorism. Journal of Politics, 69(2), 320–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kearns, E. M. & Joseph, K. Y. (2014). If torture is wrong, what about 24?: Torture and the Hollywood effect. American University School of Public Affairs Research Paper No. 2014-0001. http://ssrn.com/abstract=2483131.

  • Keith, L. C. (2002). Constitutional provisions for individual human rights: Are they more than mere window dressing. Political Research Quarterly, 55(1), 111–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keith, L. C. (2011). Political repression: Courts and the law. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khan, S. A. (2012). Sharia Law, Islamophobia and the US constitution: New tectonic plates of the culture wars. University of Maryland Law Journal of Race, Religion, Gender & Class, 12(1), 123–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kibris, A. (2011). Funerals and elections: The effects of terrorism on voting behavior in Turkey. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 55(2), 220–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R., & Sears, D. O. (1981). Prejudice and politics: Symbolic racism versus racial threats to the good life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(3), 414–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, R. D., Messner, S. F., & Baller, R. D. (2009). Contemporary hate crimes, Law enforcement, and the legacy of racial violence. American Sociological Review, 74(2), 291–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahav, G. (2004). Public opinion toward immigration in the European Union: Does it matter? Comparative Political Studies, 37(10), 1151–1183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, T. (2005). Protecting human rights: A comparative study. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luban, D. (2005). Liberalism and the unpleasant question of torture. Virginia Law Review, 91(6), 1425–1461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malhotra, N., & Popp, E. (2012). Bridging partisan divisions over antiterrorism policies: The role of threat perception. Political Research Quarterly, 65(1), 34–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malka, A., & Soto, C. J. (2011). The conflicting influences of religiosity on attitude toward torture. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8), 1091–1103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. E. (1995). With malice toward some: How people make civil liberties judgments. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., & Armor, D. J. (2012). Support for torture over time: Interrogating the American public about coercive tactics. The Social Science Journal, 49(4), 439–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Koizumi, N., & Malik, A. A. (2014). Does terror cause torture? A comparative study of international public opinion about governmental use of coercion. In T. Lightcap & J. P. Pfiffner (Eds.), Examining torture: Empirical studies of state repression (pp. 43–62). London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, A. C. (2006). Torture: Thinking about the unthinkable. In K. Greenberg (Ed.), The torture debate in America (pp. 98–110). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCoy, A. W. (2006). A question of torture: CIA interrogation, from the Cold War to the war on terror. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merolla, J. L., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2009). Democracy at risk: How terrorist threats affect the public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milkman, K. L., Akinola, M., & Chugh, D. (2015). What happens before? A field experiment exploring how pay and representation differentially shape bias on the pathway into organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(6), 1678–1712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2011). Torture approval in comparative perspective. Human Rights Review, 12(4), 441–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P., Gronke, P., & Rejali, D. (2014). Torture and public opinion: The partisan dimension. In T. Lightcap & J. P. Pfiffner (Eds.), Examining torture: Empirical studies of state repression (pp. 11–41). London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mondak, J. J., & Hurwitz, J. (2012). Examining the terror exception terrorism and commitments to civil liberties. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(2), 193–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nacos, B. L., & Torres-Reyna, O. (2007). Fueling our fears: Stereotyping, media coverage, and public opinion of Muslim Americans. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, P., Montague, K., & Marion, R. J. (2003). Framing terrorism: The news media, the government, and the public. London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, E. J., & Wong, J. (2003). Intergroup prejudice in multiethnic settings. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 567–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owens, P. B., Cunningham, D., & Ward, G. (2015). Threat, competition, and mobilizing structures: Motivational and organizational contingencies of the Civil Rights-Era Ku Klux Klan. Social Problems, 62(4), 572–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pager, D., Western, B., & Bonikowski, B. (2009). Discrimination in a low-wage labor market: A field experiment. American Sociological Review, 74(5), 777–799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, J. A. (2015). Terrorist suspect religious identity and public support for harsh interrogation and detention practices. Political Psychology, 36(6), 667–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plümper, T., & Neumayer, E. (2009). Famine mortality, rational political inactivity, and international food aid. World Development, 37(1), 50–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poe, S., & Neal Tate, C. (1994). Repression of personal integrity rights in the 1980’s: A global analysis. American Political Science Review, 88, 853–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, E. J., & Staton, J. K. (2009). Domestic judicial institutions and human rights treaty violation. International Studies Quarterly, 53(1), 149–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Przeworski, A., Stokes, S. C., & Manin, B. (1999). Democracy, accountability, and representation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quillian, L. (1995). Prejudice as a response to perceived group threat: Population composition and anti-immigrant and racial prejudice in Europe. American Sociological Review. doi:10.2307/2096296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rejali, D. (2007). Torture and democracy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, D. L. (1999). Perilous proxy: Human rights and the presence of national elections. Social Science Quarterly, 80(4), 648–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, D., Morrill, M., & Anderson, M. (2012). Some psycho-social correlates of U.S. citizen support for torture. Nordic Journal of Human Rights, 30(1), 63–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ron, J. (1997). Varying methods of state violence. International Organization, 51(2), 275–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shamir, M., & Sagiv-Schifter, T. (2006). Conflict, identity, and tolerance: Israel in the Al-Aqsa Intifada. Political Psychology, 27(4), 569–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shane, S. (2014). Backing C.I.A., Cheney revisits torture debate from Bush era. New York Times.

  • Sherif, M. (1966). In common predicament: Social psychology of intergroup conflict and cooperation. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade, S. (1981). The image of the Arab in America: Analysis of a poll on American attitudes. The Middle East Journal, 35, 143–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M. (1975). Personality and democratic politics. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Aloysius, H. (2007). When ways of life collide: Multiculturalism and its discontents in the Netherlands. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, L. (2012). Hard measures: Ex CIA head defends Post 9–11 tactics. CBS 60 Minutes . http://www.cbsnews.com/news/hard-measures-ex-cia-head-defends-post-9-11-tactics/.

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, 41(3), 157–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1996). Predicting prejudice. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20(3), 409–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (2000). An integrated threat theory of prejudice. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination (pp. 23–45). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stouffer, S. A. (1955). Communism, conformity, and civil liberties: A cross-section of the nation speaks its mind. New York: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, J. L., Piereson, J., & Marcus, G. E. (1982). Political tolerance and American democracy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, J. L., Shamir, M., Walsh, P., & Roberts, N. S. (1985). Political tolerance in context: Support for unpopular minorities in Israel, New Zealand, and the United States. Boulder, NV: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology. Cambridge, MA: CUP Archive.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33(1), 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 34–47). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. C. (1998). How white attitudes vary with the racial composition of local populations: Numbers count. American Sociological Review, 63(4), 512–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tilly, C. (2004). Terror, terrorism, terrorists. Sociological Theory, 22(1), 5–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Truman, J. S. (2003). Communicating terror: The rhetorical dimensions of terrorism. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Nations. (1984). Convention against torture. New York: United Nations Treaty Collection.

  • Vavreck, L., & Rivers, D. (2008). The 2006 cooperative congressional election study. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 18(4), 355–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viki, G. T., Osgood, D., & Phillips, S. (2013). Dehumanization and self-reported proclivity to torture prisoners of war. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 325–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, G. P. R. (2013). International law and public attitudes toward torture: An experimental study. International Organization, 67(01), 105–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, G. P. R. (2014). Martial law? Military experience, international law, and support for torture. International Studies Quarterly, 58(3), 501–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walzer, M. (1973). Political action: The problem of dirty hands. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 2(2), 160–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wemlinger, E. (2014). The gender gap and torture: Opposition to torture among men and women in the USA. Social Science Journal, 51(1), 113–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, P. (2001). Terrorism versus democracy: The liberal state response. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was partially supported by the Center for the Study of Democratic Performance and the Department of Political Science at Florida State University. We are grateful to Ana Bracic, Scott Clifford, Daniel Corstange, Darren Davis, Chris Fariss, Paul Gronke, Brandon Merrill, Kristy Pathakis, Dave Siegel, Geoffrey Wallace, Thomas Zeitzoff, audiences at the 2014 and 2016 International Studies Association Annual Meetings, and colloquia at Peace Research Institute Oslo, Arizona State University, the University of California, San Diego, and the University of Denver for useful feedback. We also thank Dennis Langley of Florida State University for his research assistance. Unfortunately, during the final stages of our work on this paper, our friend and coauthor, Will Moore, passed away. He was a remarkable colleague, mentor, and scholar; he will be greatly missed.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brad T. Gomez.

Additional information

Data and replication files are available at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/polbehavior.

Deceased: Will H. Moore.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (pdf 41 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Conrad, C.R., Croco, S.E., Gomez, B.T. et al. Threat Perception and American Support for Torture. Polit Behav 40, 989–1009 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9433-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9433-5

Keywords

Navigation