Advertisement

What I Felt Like Doing, but did Not Do When I Felt Hurt: An REBT-Based Investigation of Action Tendencies that are Not Acted On

  • Windy Dryden
  • Nicola R. Hurton
Original Article

Abstract

In this questionnaire-based study, we investigated the action tendencies that 100 people reported having, but did not act on, in specific situations in which they felt hurt. An awareness of these types of unacted-on tendencies will help rational emotive behavior therapy therapists to be alert to the possible presence of hurt feelings and hurt-related motivational states that clients do not readily report experiencing in ABC analyses of hurt-related emotional episodes.

Keywords

Hurt Unacted-on action tendencies Action tendencies Rational emotive behavior therapy 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Grant from the Research Capability Fund in the Department of Professional and Community Education, Goldsmiths, University of London.

References

  1. Bachman, G. F., & Guerrero, L. K. (2006). Relational quality and communicative responses following hurtful events in dating relationships: An expectancy violations analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 943–963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 77–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dryden, W. (1996). Overcoming anger: When anger helps and when it hurts. London: Sheldon Press.Google Scholar
  4. Dryden, W. (1997). Overcoming shame. London: Sheldon Press.Google Scholar
  5. Dryden, W. (2007). Overcoming hurt. London: Sheldon Press.Google Scholar
  6. Dryden, W. (2009a). Understanding emotional problems: The REBT perspective. Hove, East Sussex: Routledge.Google Scholar
  7. Dryden, W. (2009b). Rational emotive behaviour therapy: Distinctive features. Hove, East Sussex: Routledge.Google Scholar
  8. Dryden, W. (2010). Strange, but rational. Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books.Google Scholar
  9. Dryden, W., & Branch, R. (2008). The fundamentals of rational emotive behaviour therapy: A training handbook. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  10. Dryden, W., & Hurton, N. R. (2013). Why I did not do what I felt like doing when I felt hurt: An REBT-based investigation of reasons why hurt-based action tendencies are not acted on. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy. doi: 10.1007/s10942-013-0170-3.
  11. Feeney, J. A. (2004). Hurt feelings in couple relationships: Towards integrative models of the negative effects of hurtful events. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 487–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Feeney, J. A. (2005). Hurt feelings in couple relationships: Exploring the role of attachment and perceptions of personal injury. Personal Relationships, 12, 253–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Frijda, N. H. (2004). Emotions and action. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. H. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: Studies in emotion and social interaction (pp. 158–173). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety. New York: Oxford University Press. cited in MacDonald, 2009.Google Scholar
  16. Leary, M. R., & Springer, C. A. (2001). Hurt feelings: The neglected emotion. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships (pp. 151–175). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Leary, M. R., Springer, C., Negel, L., Ansell, E., & Evans, K. (1998). The causes, phenomenology, and consequences of hurt feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1225–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  19. MacDonald, G. (2009). Social pain and hurt feelings. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 541–555). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. McLaren, R. M., & Solomon, D. H. (2008). Appraisals and distancing responses to hurtful messages. Communication Research, 35, 339–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Sandelowski, M. (2001). Real qualitative researchers do not count: The use of numbers in qualitative research. Research in Nursing & Health, 24, 230–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Vangelisti, A. L. (2006). Hurtful interactions and the dissolution of intimacy. In M. A. Fine & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Handbook of divorce and relationship dissolution (pp. 133–152). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  24. Vangelisti, A. L. (2007). Communicating Hurt. In B. H. Spitzberg & W. R. Cupach (Eds.), The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 121–142). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  25. Vangelisti, A. L., & Crumley, L. P. (1998). Reactions to messages that hurt: The influence of relational contexts. Communication Monographs, 65, 173–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Vangelisti, A. L., & Young, S. L. (2000). When words hurt: The effects of perceived intentionality on interpersonal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 2000(17), 393–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Vangelisti, A. L., Young, S. L., Carpenter-Theune, K. E., & Alexander, A. L. (2005). Why does it hurt?: The perceived causes of hurt feelings. Communication Research, 32, 443–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.STaCS, GoldsmithsUniversity of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations