Method
Participants
One hundred and one participants aged between 19 and 62 years (M = 36.12, SD = 12.73; 65 female, 26 male) were included in this study.Footnote 5 Sample-size was determined by trying to recruit as many participants as possible to maximize power. Most participants were Caucasian (72.5%), with the remaining African American (9.9%), American Indian or Alaskan Native (9.9%), or of a different ethnicity (7.7%). Ninety-eight percent of participants completed high school; about two thirds (67.1%) completed college. Participants were recruited via university mailing lists and publicly available websites and asked to participate in research investigating everyday desires. Eligibility criteria included owning a smartphone, being older than 18 years of age, residing in the United States or Canada, and being fluent in English. Participants were compensated with $5 for completing an initial intake survey, and an additional $25 for responding to at least 35 out of 49 brief mobile surveys. Participants completing at least five mobile phone surveys and reporting at least five desires over the course of the study were included in this study. The study was approved by the university review board.Footnote 6
Procedure
Participants were informed about the general purpose of the study (i.e., to examine how people experience and deal with everyday desires) and received an overview of the study, compensation details, and eligibility criteria. After registering for the study, participants completed an intake survey including demographic information and several personality questionnaires not in the current focus (a complete list of measures is available upon request). Importantly, participants were also provided with a definition of what a desire refers to in this study, this definition being in line with an appetitive desire and not other non-grasping wants (e.g., higher order goals). This definition of desire provided to participants may not encapsulate all interpretations of ‘desire’ within Buddhism. However, it does fit with our view that the types of wants Buddhism typically regards as problematic are appetitive and grasping in nature. This broad strokes definition of desire was also important to facilitate its measurement in this study. The experience sampling phase commenced on the next day and lasted for seven consecutive days. On each day, participants received seven text messages, directing them to a brief mobile survey via an embedded hyperlink (see the following section on the experience sampling procedure). Text messages were spread across a 14-h time frame between 8 a.m. and 10 p.m., with one message being sent at a random time within each 2-h block with the additional constraint that two messages be at least 30 min apart (Hektner et al. 2007). A reminder SMS was sent in case the participant did not respond within 15 min of the original dispatch. On a small fraction of occasions (3.3%), participants clicked on a previously used link another time to complete another brief survey. These additional data points were retained for analyses. The median delay between the initial and the subsequent response was 1.39 h. After the mobile phase, participants were thanked and compensated for their efforts.
Experience-Sampling Procedure and Protocol
Because this study is exclusively focused on relations between desire (i.e., absence vs. presence), desire conflict, and momentary happiness, here we only briefly summarize the experience sampling protocol from where the data was taken (for more details, see Friese and Hofmann 2016). At the beginning of each mobile survey, participants completed a measure of state mindfulness and indicated whether they were either currently or recently (within the last 30 min) experiencing a desire. If they indicated no desire, they were asked for their momentary happiness, and the survey was over. If they indicated that they did experience a current or recent desire, they next indicated the desire domain from a list of 16 provided (i.e., food, non-alcoholic drinks, coffee, alcoholic drinks, sleep/rest, sex, media use, social contact, sport, work, leisure/hobbies, to express my anger, smoking, other substances, hygiene-related, other). They also reported on the desire’s strength, to what extent they tried to resist the desire, and whether the desire stood in conflict to other goals. They then indicated the degree to which they had enacted the desire (on a seven-point scale from not at all to totally). Finally, participants were asked about their momentary happiness, and indicated the extent to which they had experienced self-conscious emotions. For the below hypothesis tests, we only included present desire occurrences for which people indicated that they had not (yet) enacted the desire in question (i.e., acute desires).
Key Measures
Desire conflict was measured by asking participants how much the acute desire conflicted with other important goals, on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much). Desire conflict ratings were subsequently coded to indicate instances of low (0–2), medium (3–4), and high (5–6) conflict. Momentary happiness was measured by asking participants how happy they currently felt, on a Likert scale ranging from − 3 (very unhappy) to + 3 (very happy).
Analytic Procedures and Strategy
All core analyses—except descriptive raw data calculations—were conducted within a multilevel framework with random intercepts and fixed effects using the SPSS MIXED procedure for multilevel regression models in order to estimate indirect effects properly. This procedure was conducted using the SPSS syntax functionality with code and instructions made available on OSF to enable the replication of our findings. Given the expected dependency of proposed effects on level 2 units (i.e., observations within participants are expected to be correlated), a multilevel approach should elicit more accurate standard errors and reduced type-1 error rates compared to standard regression (Hox et al. 2010). Denominator degrees of freedom for the test of fixed effects were obtained by the Satterthwaite approximation.
An intercept-only model was initially constructed for momentary happiness (our only dependent variable) to examine the degree of variance at level 1 (within-person) and level 2 (between-person). This model is depicted in Eq. (1):
$$y_{{ij}} = \beta _{0} + ~u_{j} + ~e_{{ij}}$$
(1)
where \(y_{ij}\) represents momentary happiness for the ith observation in the jth participant, \(\beta_{0}\) represents the grand mean for momentary happiness, \(u_{j}\) represents the deviation in momentary happiness between the participant mean and the grand mean, and \(e_{ij}\) represents the error between the participants momentary happiness value and their overall mean. Then, for main analyses, our categorical predictors were added to this model to estimate average levels of momentary happiness for different types of desire experience. First, we estimated average levels of momentary happiness for acute desire vs. no desire situations. This model is depicted in Eq. (2):
$$y_{{ij}} = \beta _{0} + ~\beta _{1} {\mathcal{X}}_{{ij}} + u_{j} + ~e_{{ij}}$$
(2)
This equation is identical to Eq. (1), with the exception that \(\beta_{0}\) represents mean momentary happiness when there is no desire (reference category), \(\beta_{0} + u_{j}\) represents mean momentary happiness for participant j when there is no desire, and the slope \(\beta_{1}\) represents the difference in the mean momentary happiness when there is an acute desire (i.e., \({\mathcal{X}}_{ij}\) = 1) relative to when there is no desire (i.e., \({\mathcal{X}}_{ij}\) = 0). Second, we estimated average levels of momentary happiness between acute desires at low, medium, and high conflict with important personal goals, and no desire contexts. This model is depicted in Eq. (3):
$$y_{{ij}} = \beta _{0} + ~\beta _{1} {\mathcal{X}}_{{ij}} + \beta _{2} {\mathcal{X}}_{{ij}} + \beta _{3} {\mathcal{X}}_{{ij}} + u_{j} + ~e_{{ij}}$$
(3)
This equation is identical to Eq. (2), but with the exception that \(\beta_{1} {\mathcal{X}}_{ij} ,\beta_{2} {\mathcal{X}}_{ij} ,\; {\text{and}}\; \beta_{3} {\mathcal{X}}_{ij}\), represent acute desires that are, respectively, in low, medium, and high conflict with other personal goals held by the participant.
Results
Descriptive and Frequency Data
On average, participants responded to 31.92 (N = 3224, SD = 10.76, range = 7–49) out of a maximum of 49 brief surveys,Footnote 7 for a response rate of 65.14%. In total, participants reported 828 current and 642 recent desires (25.68% and 19.91% of all responses, respectively). Participants reported no desire on 1,734 occasions (53.78%). Of the current desires in the focus of the present analysis (i.e., recent desires not considered in analyses), 310 had not yet been enacted, and hence were retained as acute desires. Of those, 150 (48.39%), 65 (20.97%), and 95 (30.65%) were in Low, Medium, and High conflict (respectively) with other important personal goals. Data pertaining to desire presence was not available on 20 occasions. Mean momentary happiness per participant was 1.08 (SD = 0.82, range = −0.63 to 2.83).
Our intercept-only model for momentary happiness revealed that 26.67% of the variance in momentary happiness was at the between-person level [0.61/(1.67 + 0.61) = 0.27], with the remaining variance (i.e., 73.33%) at the within-person level (1.67/(1.67 + 0.61) = 0.73). This outcome supports our decision to control for level 2 (person-level) effects when examining hypothesized effects.
Desire and Momentary Happiness
Estimated mean momentary happiness levels by desire type are reported in Fig. 1. As expected, momentary happiness was significantly greater in instances where individuals were not experiencing a desire, compared to when they were experiencing an acute desire (b = 0.80 [Cohens d = 0.62 medium effect],Footnote 8 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.64, 0.96], SE = 0.08), t(2015.95) = 9.88, p < 0.001, Nlevel1 = 2037, Nlevel2 = 101. Furthermore, level of conflict exerted a main effect on momentary happiness, F(3, 1993.03) = 31.38, p < 0.001, Nlevel1 = 2037, Nlevel2 = 101, suggesting that the above lower momentary happiness associated with the degree of conflict with other important goals.
Specifically, momentary happiness when not experiencing a desire (baseline) was significantly greater than when an acute desire was experienced that was at low conflict (b = 0.59 [d = 0.38, medium effect], 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.38, 0.81], SE = 0.11, contrast with baseline: t(2009.03) = 5.40, p < 0.001), medium conflict (b = 0.68 [d = 0.63, medium-to-large effect], 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.37, 0.98], SE = 0.16, t(1979.48) = 4.34, p < 0.001), or high conflict (b = 1.21 [d = 0.99, large effect], 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.95, 1.47], SE = 0.13, t(1995.37) = 9.14, p < 0.001).
Brief Discussion
Study 1 provided initial support for the Buddha Hypothesis, with momentary happiness greater in the absence of desire than when experiencing an acute desire. This difference in momentary happiness was quite substantial, and likely indicative of the potent nature of negative affective signals (relative to positive affective signals) when acute desire is experienced (e.g., Kavanagh et al. 2005). Our secondary hypothesis that desire-goal conflict would qualify the negative effect of experiencing desire on momentary happiness was also supported: The greater the experienced conflict the stronger the negative effect of experiencing a desire on momentary happiness. This test was informative, as it is in principle possible that acute desires are typically in high conflict with personal goals. This was not the case; there were substantial proportions of low-conflict and medium-conflict desires. In addition, even acute desires in low conflict with personal goals were still associated with significantly lower momentary happiness as compared to no desire states. We therefore believe it is best to conceptualize conflict as an additional source of negativity, next to the state of wanting per se.