Skip to main content
Log in

How Are Traits Related to Problem Behavior in Preschoolers? Similarities and Contrasts Between Temperament and Personality

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The lack of empirical research relating temperament models and personality hinders conceptual integration and holds back research linking childhood traits to problem behavior or maladjustment. This study evaluates, within a sample of 443 preschoolers, the relationships between children’s maladaptation and traits measured by three temperament models (Thomas and Chess, Buss and Plomin, and Rothbart), and a Five-Factor based personality model. Adequate reliabilities and expected factor structures are demonstrated for most scales. A joint principal component analysis combining 28 temperament and 18 personality scales indicates a six-factor model, distinguishing Sociability, Activity, Conscientiousness, Disagreeableness, Emotionality, and Sensitivity. Regression analyses reveal that although single temperament and personality scales explain from 23% to 37% of problem behavior variance, the six components explain from 41% to 49% and provide a clearer differentiation among CBCL-problem scales. This age-specific taxonomy refines and corroborates conclusions based on narrative reviews and furnishes a more balanced view of trait–maladjustment relationships.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the Flemish community of Belgium, almost 100% of the children access kindergarten from age 3.

  2. Consequently, analyses including these temperament scales are based on a varying N.

  3. Analyses of variance revealed no significant mean differences (p < 0.05) between mothers and fathers ratings of problem behavior. Adopting the p < .01 level, no differences were found between paternal and maternal ratings of temperament and personality. When the threshold was set to p < 0.05, two differences were found: Fathers rated their preschoolers lower on CBQ-Effortful Control (F(1, 265) = 5.54, p = 0.02) and on BSQ-Threshold/Distractibility (F(1, 246) = 5.97, p = 0.02).

  4. Reports of the described analyses are available upon request with the first author.

  5. Given the low internal consistencies of the BSQ-Threshold and BSQ-Regularity scale, it is possible that these two scales load on a common factor due to their shared, low reliability. Secondary principal component analyses were conducted excluding these two scales, but this did not alter the results as the second BSQ-component still combines Distractibility and Intensity of reaction. Principal axis factoring yielded an identical factor solution.

  6. In line with previous empirical research on the common structure of temperament and personality (Angleitner and Ostendorf 1994; Digman and Shmelyov 1996), we applied principal component analysis with varimax rotation in order to create orthogonal trait components and to avoid multicollinearity of predictors in the subsequent multiple regressions. Correlation coefficients for each pair of variables were computed based on all cases with valid data for that pair (N = 248–432).

  7. An additional joint principal component analysis, excluding the 23 children with an elevated score on the CBCL Total Problems scale, did not alter the documented six-factor solution. In subsequent analyses, we omitted children with significantly elevated scores on CBCL broadband scales. Again, dropping these children did not yield major changes of factor structure.

  8. The path analyses are only based on Sample 2 data.

  9. In order to find alternative paths, we also adopted a bottom-up approach sequentially dropping arrows from the fully-saturated model. In addition, modification indices were inspected that allowed to improve the fit of the model. In this way, we identified one competitive model which included an additional path between Conscientiousness and Internalizing. Goodness-of-fit indices were almost equal to the original model, but the ratio χ 2/df was less favorable for this model (0.44) than for the original model (0.74). Both models did not differ significantly from a model where all paths between traits and psychopathology were freed: original top-down model: χ 2Δ (27, N = 186) = 19.96, p > 0.05, alternative model: χ 2Δ (26, N = 186) = 11.54, p > 0.05. For reasons of parsimony and fit excellence, in addition to its theoretical basis, the original model was preferred.

References

  • Abe, J. A. A. (2005). The predictive validity of the Five-Factor Model of personality with preschool age children: A nine year follow-up study. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 423–442. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abe, J. A. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). A longitudinal study of emotion expression and personality relations in early development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 566–577. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.566.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1994). Temperament and the Big Five factors of personality. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 69–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asendorpf, J. B., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2006). Predictive validity of personality types versus personality dimensions from early childhood to adulthood: Implications for the distinction between core and surface traits. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly-Journal of Developmental Psychology, 52, 486–513. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0022.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, J. E. (1990). Conceptual and empirical linkages between temperament and behavior problems: A commentary on the Sanson, Prior, and Kyrios study. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 36, 193–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the 5-Factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boer, F., & Westenberg, P. M. (1994). The factor structure of the Buss and Plomin EAS Temperament Survey (parental ratings) in a Dutch sample of elementary-school-children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 62, 537–551. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6203_13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of personality development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 300–364, 6th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453–484. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Fruyt, F., Mervielde, I., Hoekstra, H. A., & Rolland, J. P. (2000). Assessing adolescents’ personality with the NEO PI-R. Assessment, 7, 329–345. doi:10.1177/107319110000700403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Digman, J. M., & Inouye, J. (1986). Further specification of the 5 robust factors of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 116–123. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.1.116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Digman, J. M., & Shmelyov, A. G. (1996). The structure of temperament and personality in Russian children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 341–351. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eaton, W. (1994). Temperament, development, and the five-factor model: Lessons from activity level. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 173–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75, 25–46. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00652.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 868–888. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J. (2004). Integrating research on temperament and childhood psychopathology: Its pitfalls and promise. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 2–7. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 64–86. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, M. V., Reeves, D., & Cunningham, C. C. (1987). The application of temperament questionnaires to a British sample - Issues of reliability and validity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 28, 61–77. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1987.tb00652.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative description of personality—The Big-5 factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Round-table—What is temperament 4 approaches. Child Development, 58, 505–529. doi:10.2307/1130527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, H. H., Riesen-Danner, L. A., & Briggs, S. (1991). Evaluating convergent and discriminant validity of temperament questionnaires for preschoolers, toddlers, and infants. Developmental Psychology, 27, 566–579. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.4.566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halverson, C. F., Havill, V. L., Deal, J., Baker, S. R., Victor, J. B., Pavlopoulos, V., et al. (2003). Personality structure as derived from parental ratings of free descriptions of children: The inventory of child individual differences. Journal of Personality, 71, 995–1026. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., Caspi, A., Robins, R. W., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). The little 5—exploring the nomological network of the 5-Factor Model of personality in adolescent boys. Child Development, 65, 160–178. doi:10.2307/1131373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. E., Chuang, S. S., Wessels, H., Broberg, A. G., & Hwang, C. P. (2002). Emergence and construct validation of the big five factors in early childhood: A longitudinal analysis of their ontogeny in Sweden. Child Development, 73, 1517–1524. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leenders, F., van Tijen, N., van der Vlugt, E., & Super, C. M. (1992). Dutch translation of the Behavior Styles Questionnaire for children aged 3 to 7. Amsterdam: OOS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Revealing the relation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding: Expert ratings and factor analyses. Child Development, 73, 867–882. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L. J., West, S. G., & Sandler, I. N. (1998). Temperament as a predictor of symptomatology in children: Addressing contamination of measures. Child Development, 69, 164–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Vasey, M. W., Phillips, B. M., & Hazen, R. A. (2004). Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 8–20. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Majdandzic, M., & van den Boom, D. C. (2007). Multimethod longitudinal assessment of temperament in early childhood. Journal of Personality, 75, 121–167. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00435.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R. P., Wisenbaker, J., & Huttunen, M. (1994). Review of factor analytic studies of temperament measures based on the Thomas–Chess structural model: Implications for the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 157–172). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the 5-Factor Model of Personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hrebickova, M., Avia, M. D., et al. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality, and life span development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 173–186. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. (1978). Measurement of temperament in 3–7 year old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 19, 245–253. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1978.tb00467.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Measelle, J. R., John, O. P., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2005). Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid self-reports on the Big Five dimensions? A longitudinal study from ages 5 to 7. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 90–106. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mervielde, I., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2000). Variable-centered versus person-centered approaches to childhood personality. In S. E. Hampson (Ed.), Advances in personality psychology, vol. 1 (pp. 37–76). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mervielde, I., & De Fruyt, F. (2002). Assessing children’s traits with the hierarchical personality inventory for children. In B. De Raad, & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big five assessment (pp. 129–146). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mervielde, I., Buyst, V., & De Fruyt, F. (1995). The validity of the Big-5 as a model for teachers ratings of individual-differences among children aged 4–12 years. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 525–534. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)00175-R.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mervielde, I., De Clercq, B., De Fruyt, F., & Van Leeuwen, K. (2005). Temperament, personality, and developmental psychopathology as childhood antecedents of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 171–201. doi:10.1521/pedi.19.2.171.62627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mervielde, I., De Clercq, B., De Fruyt, F., & Van Leeuwen, K. (2006). Temperament and personality as broad-spectrum antecedents of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. In T. A. Widiger, E. Simonsen, P. J. Sirovatka, & D. A. Regier (Eds.), Dimensional models of personality disorders. Refining the research agenda for DSM-V (pp. 85–109). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 271–289. doi:10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Blijlevens, P. (2007). Self-reported reactive and regulative temperament in early adolescence: Relations to internalizing and externalizing problem behavior and “Big Three” personality factors. Journal of Adolescence, 30, 1035–1049. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.03.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G., Licht, B. G., & Pearl, R. A. (1982). Some dangers of using personality questionnaires to study personality. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 572–580. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.92.3.572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 47, 395–422. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinzie, P., Onghena, P., & Hellinckx, W. (2005). Parent and child personality traits and children’s externalizing problem behavior from age 4 to 9 years: A cohort-sequential latent growth curve analysis. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly-Journal of Developmental Psychology, 51, 335–366. doi:10.1353/mpq.2005.0017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M. (1992). Childhood temperament. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 33, 249–279. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00863.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 102–112. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rettew, D. C., & McKee, L. (2005). Temperament and its role in developmental psychopathology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 13, 14–27. doi:10.1080/10673220590923146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 207–212. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00505.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 105–176, 5th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 99–166, 6th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2006). Temperament, attention, and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: vol. 2. Developmental Neuroscience (pp. 465–501, 2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–1408. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Kyrios, M. (1990). Contamination of measures in temperament research. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly-Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36, 179–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiner, R. (1998). How shall we speak of children’s personalities in middle childhood? A preliminary taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 308–332. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44, 2–32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M., Axia, G., Harkness, S., Welles-Nyström, B., Zylicz, P. O., Parmar, P., et al. (2008). Culture, temperament, and the “difficult child” in seven Western cultures. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2, 136–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tackett, J. L. (2006). Evaluating models of the personality-psychopathology relationship in children and adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 584–599. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zupancic, M., Podlesek, A., & Kavcic, T. (2006). Personality types as derived from parental reports on 3-year-old. European Journal of Personality, 20, 285–303. doi:10.1002/per.584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

This study was supported by a Ph.D. research grant awarded by the Research Foundation Flanders to the first author (Grant nr: 1.1.212.08.N.01) under supervision of the second author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah S. W. De Pauw.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

De Pauw, S.S.W., Mervielde, I. & Van Leeuwen, K.G. How Are Traits Related to Problem Behavior in Preschoolers? Similarities and Contrasts Between Temperament and Personality. J Abnorm Child Psychol 37, 309–325 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9290-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9290-0

Keywords

Navigation