Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 40, Issue 2, pp 164–172 | Cite as

Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem in Previously and Never Depressed Individuals: Baseline Differences and Reactivity to Rumination

  • John E. Roberts
  • Andrew Porter
  • Chrystal Vergara-Lopez
Original Article

Abstract

In contrast to cognitive theories that emphasize negative self-views in depression, a recent study demonstrated that previously depressed individuals have higher implicit self-esteem (SE) than never depressed controls (Franck et al. in Cogn Emot 22(8):1588–1599, 2008). The present study was designed to extend this past work by measuring both implicit and explicit SE among previously and never depressed individuals and testing whether a rumination manipulation impacts SE more strongly among individuals with past depression. Previously (n = 28) and never depressed (n = 33) participants completed rumination and distraction conditions in sessions separated by 1 week. State explicit SE and implicit SE were measured both pre- and post-manipulation, whereas explicit trait SE was measured once at baseline. Previously depressed individuals had higher implicit SE, but lower explicit trait SE, at baseline compared to never depressed controls. Never depressed individuals experienced decreases in implicit SE following rumination relative to distraction, whereas previously depressed individuals experienced decreases in implicit SE following both rumination and distraction. Our findings demonstrate that previously depressed individuals have high implicit, but low explicit trait SE, compared to never depressed persons, and further suggest that they differ in either their response to repeated administrations of measures of implicit SE or to externally prompted shifts in attentional focus.

Keywords

Implicit self-esteem Explicit self-esteem Rumination Depression 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Jan De Houwer for his insightful comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

John E. Roberts, Andrew Porter, and Chrystal Vergara-Lopez declared that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standard

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (national and institutional). Informed consent was obtained from all individual subjects participating in the study. If any identifying information is contained in the paper the following statement is also necessary—Additional informed consent was obtained from any subjects for whom identifying information appears in this paper.

Animal Rights

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

References

  1. Beevers, C. G. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to depression: A dual process model. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(7), 975–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.03.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B, Jr, & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect of measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 631–643. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.631.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Butler, A., Hokanson, J., & Flynn, H. (1994). A comparison of self-esteem lability and low trait self-esteem as vulnerability factors for depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 166–177. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.166.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Cannon, D. S., Tiffany, S. T., Coon, H., Scholan, M. B., McMahon, W. M., & Leppert, M. F. (2007). The PHQ-9 as brief assessment of lifetime major depression. Psychological Assessment, 19(2), 247–251. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.247.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Ciesla, J. A., Felton, J. W., & Roberts, J. E. (2011). Testing the Cognitive Catalyst Model of depression: Does rumination amplify the impact of cognitive diatheses in response to stress? Cognition and Emotion, 25, 1349–1357.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Ciesla, J. A., & Roberts, J. E. (2007). Rumination, negative cognition, and their interactive effects on depressed mood: Two laboratory studies. Emotion, 7, 555–565.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. De Raedt, R., Schacht, R., Franck, E., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Self-esteem and depression revisited: Implicit positive self-esteem in depressed patients? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(7), 1017–1028. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Franck, E., & De Raedt, R. (2007). Self-esteem reconsidered: Unstable self-esteem outperforms level of self-esteem as vulnerability marker for depression. Behavior Research and Therapy, 45(10), 1531–1541. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Franck, E., De Raedt, R., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Implicit but not explicit self-esteem predicts future depressive symptomatology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(10), 2448–2455. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Franck, E., De Raedt, R., & De Houwer, J. (2008). Activation of latent self-schemas as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression: The potential role of implicit self-esteem. Cognition and Emotion, 22(8), 1588–1599. doi: 10.1080/02699930801921271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gamble, S. A., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Roberts, J. E., Ciesla, J. A., & Pelham, W. E. (2013). Self-esteem reactivity among mothers of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): The moderating role of depression history. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37, 1233–1242.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Gemar, M., Segal, Z., Sagrati, S., & Kennedy, S. (2001). Mood-induced changes on the Implicit Association Test in recovered depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 282–289. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.282.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Greenwald, A., & Farnham, S. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 1022–1038. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Haeffel, G. J., Abramson, L. Y., Brazy, P. C., Shah, J. Y., Teachman, B. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2007). Explicit and implicit cognition: A preliminary test of a dual-process theory of cognitive vulnerability to depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(6), 1155–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hartlage, S., Arduino, K., & Alloy, L. B. (1998). Depressive personality characteristics: State dependent concomitants of depressive disorder and traits independent of current depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(2), 349–354. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.2.349.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hoorens, V., Takano, K., Franck, E., Roberts, J. E., & Raes, F. (2015). Initial and noninitial name-letter preferences as obtained through repeated letter rating tasks continue to reflect (different aspects of) self-esteem. Psychological Assessment, 27(3), 905–914. doi: 10.1037/pas0000092.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Inquisit (version 3.0.5.0) [Computer software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software LLC.Google Scholar
  20. Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Luxton, D. D., & Wenzlaff, R. M. (2005). Self-esteem uncertainty and depression vulnerability. Cognition and Emotion, 19(4), 611–622. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 166–177. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.166.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7(6), 561–570. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives of Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Core Team (2014). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-117. http://CRAN.Rproject.org/package=nlme.
  27. R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/.
  28. Remue, J., De Houwer, J., Barnes-Holmes, D., Vanderhasselt, M., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Self-esteem revisited: Performance on the implicit relational assessment procedure as a measure of self- versus ideal self-related cognitions in dysphoria. Cognition and Emotion, 27(8), 1441–1449. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.786681.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Risch, A. K., Buba, A., Birk, U., Morina, N., Steffens, M. C., & Stangier, U. (2010). Implicit self-esteem in recurrently depressed patients. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(3), 199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.01.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Roberts, J. E., & Monroe, S. M. (1994). A multidimensional model of self-esteem in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 14(3), 161–181. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(94)90006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P., & Seeley, J. (1990). Are people changed by the experience of having an episode of depression? A further test of the scar hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(3), 264–271. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.99.3.264.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Serretti, A., Cavallini, M. C., Macciardi, F., Namia, C., Franchini, L., Souery, D., et al. (1999). Social adjustment and self-esteem in remitted patients with mood disorders. European Psychiatry, 14(3), 137–142. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(99)80731-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Shapira, B., Zislin, J., Gelfin, Y., Osher, Y., Gorfine, M., Souery, D., et al. (1999). Social adjustment and self-esteem in remitted patients with unipolar and bipolar affective disorder: A case–control study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 40(1), 24–30. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(99)90072-X.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., & Williams, J. W. (1999). Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: The PHQ Primary Care Study. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 282(18), 1737–1744. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2(3), 247–274. doi: 10.1080/02699938808410927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • John E. Roberts
    • 1
  • Andrew Porter
    • 1
  • Chrystal Vergara-Lopez
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity at Buffalo, The State University of New YorkBuffaloUSA

Personalised recommendations