Skip to main content
Log in

A mathematical model of social selection favoring reduced aggression

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Emergence of characteristically human traits, including extensive collaboration, prosociality, and cooperative communication, has presumably necessitated evolution toward lower aggression. While empirical studies have provided evidence for evolutionary reduction in human aggression, such as canine teeth reduction in hominins and craniofacial feminization in Homo sapiens, the underlying mechanisms for the reduction in human aggression are virtually unknown. A promising hypothesis is social selection by partner choice; that is, individuals’ choice of collaborative partners may have induced selection against exceedingly aggressive individuals. However, it is thus far unclear whether partner choice can give unaggressive individuals a fitness benefit that more than compensates for the lost advantage of being aggressive in interindividual conflict. This study explores the plausibility of the social selection hypothesis, using a game-theoretic model of coalition formation. Analysis of the game shows that partner choice can induce selection favoring reduced fighting ability if individuals are ecologically demanded to collaborate, they can be viewed as rational fitness maximizers, and they have communicative skills to bring a synergy in collaboration. The model also suggests a possible feedback loop between reduction of fighting ability and a correlated enhancement of communicative skills.

Significance statement

Domesticated animals often share a suite of morphological, physiological, and cognitive characteristics, called the domestication syndrome, that are distinct from their wild ancestors. Experimental studies have shown that the domestication syndrome can arise as a by-product of selection against aggressiveness. A similar process may occur in wild animals; in particular, it has been suggested that “self-domestication” through selection against aggression may have played a major role in human evolution. Despite the proposed role of reduced aggression, however, it is thus far unclear in what ecological context less aggressive individuals can be selectively favored over more aggressive ones. This study provides a theoretical foundation for the social selection hypothesis for the evolution of reduced aggression, using a game-theoretic model of coalition formation, and specifies the conditions under which individuals’ choice of collaborative partners can induce selection favoring reduced aggression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Not applicable.

References

  • Archetti M (2011) Survival of the weakest in N-person duels and the maintenance of variation under constant selection. Evolution 66:637–650

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benítez-Burraco A, Kempe V (2018) The emergence of modern languages: has human self-domestication optimized language transmission? Front Psychol 9:551

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton D (2009) Adam’s tongue: how humans made language, how language made humans. Hill and Wang, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton D, Szathmáry E (2011) Confrontational scavenging as a possible source for language and cooperation. BMC Evol Biol 11:261

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bissonnette A, Perry S, Barrett L, Mitani JC, Flinn M, Gavrilets S, de Waal FBM (2015) Coalitions in theory and reality: a review of pertinent variables and processes. Behaviour 152:1–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm C (1997) Impact of the human egalitarian syndrome on Darwinian selection mechanics. Am Nat 150:S100–S121

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm C (2012) Moral origins: the evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm C (2014) The moral consequences of social selection. Behaviour 151:167–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S (2009) Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science 324:1293–1298

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. P Natl Acad Sci USA 100:3531–3535

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Broom M, Koenig A, Borries C (2009) Variation in dominance hierarchies among group-living animals: modeling stability and the likelihood of coalitions. Behav Ecol 20:844–855

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplow T (1956) A theory of coalitions in the triad. Am Sociol Rev 21:489–493

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplow T (1959) Further development of a theory of coalitions in the triad. Am J Sociol 64:488–493

    Google Scholar 

  • Cieri RL, Churchill SE, Franciscus RG, Tan J, Hare B (2014) Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. Curr Anthropol 4:419–443

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor R, Whitehead H (2005) Alliances II. Rates of encounter during resource utilization: a general model of intrasexual alliance formation in fission-fusion societies. Anim Behav 69:127–132

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (2011) A role for relaxed selection in the evolution of the language capacity. P Natl Acad Sci USA 107:9000–9006

    Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin LA (1998) A model of coalition formation in animals. Proc R Soc Lond B 265:2121–2125

    Google Scholar 

  • Galbany J, Tung J, Altmann J, Alberts SC (2015) Canine length in wild male baboons: maturation, aging and social dominance rank. PLoS One 10:e0126415

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gallotti M, Frith CD (2013) Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends Cogn Sci 17:160–165

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gavrilets S (2012) On the evolutionary origins of the egalitarian syndrome. P Natl Acad Sci USA 109:14069–14074

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gavrilets S, Duenez-Guzman EA, Vose MD (2008) Dynamics of alliance formation and the egalitarian revolution. PLoS One 3:e3293

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gleeson BT, Kushnick G (2018) Female status, food security, and stature sexual dimorphism: testing mate choice as a mechanism in human self-domestication. Am J Phys Anthropol 167:458–469

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B (2017) Survival of the friendliest: Homos sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Ann Rev Psychol 68:155–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Plyusnina I, Ignacio N, Schepina O, Stepika A, Wrangham R, Trut L (2005) Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Curr Biol 15:226–230

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Wobber V, Wrangham R (2012) The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Anim Behav 83:573–585

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey PH, Kavanagh M, Clutton-Brock TH (1978) Sexual dimorphism in primate teeth. J Zool Lond 186:475–485

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone RA, Dugatkin LA (2000) Coalition formation in animals and the nature of winner and loser effects. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:17–21

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krupenye C, Call J (2019) Theory of mind in animals: current and future directions. WIREs Cogn Sci 10:e1503

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach HM (2003) Human domestication reconsidered. Curr Anthropol 44:349–368

    Google Scholar 

  • Leigh SR, Setchell JM, Charpentier M, Knapp LA, Wickings EJ (2008) Canine tooth size and fitness in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). J Hum Evol 55:75–85

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovejoy CO (2009) Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science 326:74e1–74e8

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon BE, Montgomerie R (2012) Sexual selection is a form of social selection. Phil Trans R Soc B 367:2266–2273

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melis AP, Hare B, Tomasello M (2006) Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science 311:1297–1300

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mesterton-Gibbons M, Sherratt TN (2007) Coalition formation: a game-theoretic analysis. Behav Ecol 18:277–286

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesterton-Gibbons M, Gavrilets S, Gravner J, Akçay E (2011) Models of coalition or alliance formation. J Theor Biol 274:187–204

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura M, Hosaka K, Itoh N, Matsumoto T, Matsusaka T, Nakazawa N, Nishie H, Sakamaki T, Shimada M, Takahata Y, Yamagami M, Zamma K (2019) Wild chimpanzees deprived a leopard of its kill: implications for the origin of hominin confrontational scavenging. J Hum Evol 131:129–138

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakao H, Tamura K, Arimatsu Y, Nakagawa T, Matsumoto N, Matsugi T (2016) Violence in the prehistoric period of Japan: the spatio-temporal pattern of skeletal evidence of violence in the Jomon period. Biol Lett 12:20160028

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Noë R (1994) A model of coalition formation among male baboons with fighting ability as the crucial parameter. Anim Behav 47:211–213

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Okanoya K (2015) Evolution of song complexity in Bengalese finches could mirror the emergence of human language. J Ornithol 156:S65–S72

    Google Scholar 

  • Pandit SM, van Schaik CP (2003) A model for leveling coalitions among primate males: toward a theory of egalitarianism. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:161–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Plavcan JM, van Schaik CP (1992) Intrasexual competition and canine dimorphism in anthropoid primates. Am J Phys Anthropol 87:461–477

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plavcan JM, van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM (1995) Competition, coalitions and canine size in primates. J Hum Evol 28:245–276

    Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez-Villagra MR, van Schaik CP (2019) Evaluating the self-domestication hypothesis of human evolution. Evol Anthropol 28:133–143

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stamatopoulos G, Sengupta A, Vogel E, Janson C (2009) A game-theoretic model of coalition formation among primates. J Bioecon 11:165–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Suwa G, Kono RT, Simpson SW, Asfaw B, Lovejoy CO, White TD (2009) Paleobiological implications of the Ardipithecus ramidus dentition. Science 326:94–99

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor PD, Jonker LB (1978) Evolutionarily stable strategies and game dynamics. Math Biosci 40:145–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Theofanopoulou C, Gastaldon S, O’Rourke T, Samuels BD, Messner A, Martins PT, Delogu F, Alamri S, Boeckx C (2017) Self-domestication in Homo sapiens: insights from comparative genomics. PLoS One 12:e0185306

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas J, Kirby S (2018) Self domestication and the evolution of language. Biol Philos 33:9

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tokuyama N, Furuichi T (2016) Do friends help each other? Patterns of female coalition formation in wild bonobos at Wamba. Anim Behav 119:27–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2016) A natural history of human mortality. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intensions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28:675–735

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Melis AP, Tennie C, Wyman E, Herrmann E (2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation. Curr Anthropol 53:673–692

    Google Scholar 

  • Trut L, Oskina I, Kharlamova A (2009) Animal evolution during domestication: the domesticated fox as a model. BioEssays 31:349–360

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik CP, Pandit SA, Vogel MW (2004) A model for within-group coalitionary aggression among males. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:101–109

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (1983) Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. Q Rev Biol 58:155–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead H, Connor R (2005) Alliances I. How large should alliances be? Anim Behav 69:117–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham RW (2018) Two types of aggression in human evolution. P Natl Acad Sci USA 115:245–253

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (2019a) The goodness paradox: how evolution made us both more and less violent. Profile Books, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham RW (2019b) Hypotheses for the evolution of reduced reactive aggression in the context of human self-domestication. Front Psychol 10:1914

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Code availability

C code for simulation is available upon request.

Funding

This work was supported by the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI (Grant Number JP17H06381).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

YI designed the research, did the analyses, and wrote the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yasuo Ihara.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author declares that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Communicated by M. Raymond

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 196 kb)

Appendix. Further details of the three-player coalition game

Appendix. Further details of the three-player coalition game

We assume that the probabilities, hS, hT, and hN, that the helper supports the starter, target, or none, respectively, are described by a logit model, namely,

$$ \left(\begin{array}{c}{h}_{\mathrm{S}}\\ {}{h}_{\mathrm{T}}\\ {}{h}_{\mathrm{H}}\end{array}\right)=\frac{1}{\varPhi}\left(\begin{array}{c}\exp \left[\alpha {w}_{\mathrm{H}\mid \mathrm{R}=\mathrm{S}}\right]\\ {}\exp \left[\alpha {w}_{\mathrm{H}\mid \mathrm{R}=\mathrm{T}}\right]\\ {}\exp \left[\alpha {w}_{\mathrm{H}\mid \mathrm{R}=\mathrm{N}}\right]\end{array}\right),\kern1em $$
(5)

where Φ = exp[αwH|R=S] + exp[αwH|R=T] + exp[αwH|R=N]. In (5), wH|R=ϕ denotes the helper’s winning probability given R = ϕ (Table 1), and α measures the extent to which the helper’s decision depends on the winning probabilities (α ≥ 0). When α = 0, the helper is equally likely to take each of the three choices, while when α → ∞, the helper always takes the option maximizing the prospect of winning.

The starter is assumed to have the access to the winning probability wS, formally given by wS(aS, aT, aH) = hSwS|R=S + hTwS|R=T + hNwS|R=N, as a function of aS, aT, and aH. Consider the trio of individuals X, Y, and Z, with fighting abilities x, y, and z, respectively. To be specific, suppose that X is chosen to be the starter, so that aS = x. The starter chooses a target on the basis of a comparison between wS(x, y, z) and wS(x, z, y), whereby determines the values for aT and aH. Let gY|S=X and gZ|S=X denote the probabilities with which the starter X chooses Y or Z as the target, respectively. We assume that these probabilities follow the logit model given by

$$ \left(\begin{array}{c}{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=X}\\ {}{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=X}\end{array}\right)=\frac{1}{\Psi_X}\left(\begin{array}{c}\exp \left[\beta {w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(x,y,z\right)\right]\\ {}\exp \left[\beta {w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(x,z,y\right)\right]\end{array}\right),\kern1em $$
(6)

where ΨX = exp[βwS(x, y, z)] + exp[βwS(x, z, y)]. Here, β is a measure of the extent to which the starter’s decision depends on the winning probabilities (β ≥ 0). When β = 0, then gY|S=X = gZ|S=X = 1/2, whereas when β → ∞, the starter always takes the option leading to the highest winning probability.

Considering that each individual is chosen to be the starter with probability 1/3, the unconditional winning probabilities for X, Y, and Z are given by

$$ {w}_X=\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=X}{w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(x,y,z\right)+{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=X}{w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(x,z,y\right)\right]+\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{X\mid \mathrm{S}=Y}{w}_{\mathrm{T}}\left(y,x,z\right)+{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=Y}{w}_{\mathrm{H}}\left(y,z,x\right)\right]+\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{X\mid \mathrm{S}=Z}{w}_{\mathrm{T}}\left(z,x,y\right)+{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=Z}{w}_{\mathrm{H}}\left(z,y,x\right)\right],\kern1em $$
(7a)
$$ {w}_Y=\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=X}{w}_{\mathrm{T}}\left(x,y,z\right)+{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=X}{w}_{\mathrm{H}}\left(x,z,y\right)\right]+\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{X\mid \mathrm{S}=Y}{w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(y,x,z\right)+{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=Y}{w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(y,z,x\right)\right]+\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{X\mid \mathrm{S}=Z}{w}_{\mathrm{H}}\left(z,x,y\right)+{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=Z}{w}_{\mathrm{T}}\left(z,y,x\right)\right],\kern1em $$
(7b)
$$ {w}_Z=\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=X}{w}_{\mathrm{H}}\left(x,y,z\right)+{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=X}{w}_{\mathrm{T}}\left(x,z,y\right)\right]+\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{X\mid \mathrm{S}=Y}{w}_{\mathrm{H}}\left(y,x,z\right)+{g}_{Z\mid \mathrm{S}=Y}{w}_{\mathrm{T}}\left(y,z,x\right)\right]+\frac{1}{3}\left[{g}_{X\mid \mathrm{S}=Z}{w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(z,x,y\right)+{g}_{Y\mid \mathrm{S}=Z}{w}_{\mathrm{S}}\left(z,y,x\right)\right],\kern1em $$
(7c)

where gX|S=Y, gZ|S=Y, gX|S=Z, and gY|S=Z are defined in a manner analogous to (6), wT(aS, aT, aH) = hSwT|R=S + hTwT|R=T + hNwT|R=N, and wH(aS, aT, aH) = hSwH|R=S + hTwH|R=T + hNwH|R=N.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ihara, Y. A mathematical model of social selection favoring reduced aggression. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 74, 91 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02875-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02875-4

Keywords

Navigation