Introduction

In the next decades, urban planners and managers will increasingly need to address urban heat problems because of rising temperatures, more frequent and more extreme heat events, and the urban heat island effect (EEA 2016; IPCC 2014). Also for the Netherlands, heat stress in cities is expected to become an important issue (Molenaar et al. 2015; Steeneveld et al. 2011). However, several urban designers and planners in the Netherlands have indicated that they are struggling with this issue, since it is insufficiently clear to what extent urban design measures can reduce heat stress in Dutch urban areas, and which outdoor urban environments are most comfortable during hot days.

To contribute to this discussion and to be able to advise urban designers and planners which cool and comfortable urban design measures they can take to adapt cities to urban heat, we assessed the thermal conditions of urban spaces. Detailed thermal assessments of outdoor urban spaces normally imply fieldwork studies in which micrometeorological investigations are combined with questionnaire surveys to evaluate the thermal effect in terms of air temperature, thermal comfort, and thermal perception (Lenzholzer et al. 2016).

Many thermal assessments of outdoor urban spaces and urban designs have been carried out in the recent past, especially in countries other than the Netherlands (e.g. Lindberg et al. 2016; Middel et al. 2016; Yahia and Johansson 2013; Thorsson et al. 2004). These studies might therefore not be applicable to the Dutch urban situation or the Dutch climate. Thermal assessments that have been carried out in Dutch urban spaces (e.g. Van Hove et al. 2015; Taleghani et al. 2015; Theeuwes et al. 2013) often focus on the physical thermal conditions only and exclude how people experience and value the thermal conditions. Actually, it was Lenzholzer (e.g. Lenzholzer and Koh 2010; Lenzholzer 2010) and Klemm (e.g. Klemm et al. 2015a, 2015b) who started to assess the thermal situations of Dutch urban spaces using a combination of micrometeorological measurements and interview surveys to evaluate the thermal experience. However, in their studies often only one type of urban space was assessed (squares or green spaces respectively). A study in which the microclimatic effects and thermal experience of multiple types of urban spaces, such as green and shaded areas and locations near water bodies, are compared to each other has not yet been carried out in Netherlands.

The main aim of this study is therefore to assess the thermal conditions of different urban spaces in Amsterdam and to identify which urban spaces are experienced and measured as most comfortable during hot summer days. We combined micrometeorological measurements from mobile weather stations with an extensive questionnaire survey to assess the thermal conditions of three types of urban spaces: impervious urban spaces, green urban spaces and urban spaces close to water bodies. All locations could be either sunlit or shaded by trees or buildings. The fieldwork was carried out between noon and late afternoon during 12 hot days in 2015 and 2016.

This study was accomplished within the framework of a Dutch applied research project ‘Urban climate resilience - Turning climate adaptation into practice’, which was concluded in 2016. The project aimed to support urban professionals and closely cooperate with urban professionals. The professionals in this project were especially interested in understanding which spaces in cities remain cool and comfortable during hot days in order to take this into account in their planning strategies and designs. They particularly wanted to know which urban spaces can generate substantial cooling. In the second year of the project, the professionals were especially interested in the cooling effect of water bodies in cities. Therefore, the analyses carried out in 2016 focused on the thermal effect of urban water bodies only.

In addition to assessing the thermal situation of different urban spaces in Amsterdam, we evaluate the added value of using questionnaire surveys in this paper. Are the micrometeorological measurements consistent or inconsistent with the results of the surveys, do they provide the same insight, do they contradict or complement each other? Do the results from the surveys add relevant information to the measurements in advising urban planners how to design thermally comfortable spaces? These questions will be touched on in the ‘Discussion’ section.

Materials and methods

Measurement locations

The urban spaces of which the thermal conditions were assessed are all located in Amsterdam. Amsterdam is the capital and the most populous city of the Netherlands. The number of inhabitants is nearly 850,000 (CBS 2017). The climate of Amsterdam is a temperate marine climate (Köppen climate classification Cfb) and summers are normally moderately cool.

Nearly all measurement sites are located in the centre of Amsterdam (Fig. 1). They were selected because they are well-known and well-visited spaces. Well-visited locations are useful to collect a considerable number of surveys. The reason to select also well-known spaces is to enable the dissemination of the results to urban professionals, which is easier if they know the spaces and recognise the characteristics.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of the measurement sites. The colour indicates the type of location: grey are impervious urban spaces, green are parks with grass, shrubs and trees, and blue are locations near urban water. The locations are numbered in alphabetical order. *Data measured at Oosterdok are not used in this paper

We assessed the thermal conditions of three types of urban spaces: (1) grey locations—impervious areas such as streets and squares, (2) green locations—green spaces such as parks existing of grass, shrubs and trees, and (3) blue locations—urban areas close to water bodies such as canals, rivers, ponds and fountains. Each location was either sunlit or in the shade of trees or buildings. On the basis of visual observations, we determined if a location was in the sun or in the shade. Locations were sunlit or shaded during the total measurement period. The measurement locations at the blue locations were always at the downwind direction of the water body and within a distance of 8 m from the water body. In total, 13 impervious urban spaces, 2 green urban spaces and 6 spaces near urban water bodies were assessed.

Table 1 gives an overview of the measured sites in Amsterdam, including the measurement date and period, the location type, the situation: sunlit or shaded, a description of the site and the Local Climate Zone (Stewart and Oke 2012) that we identified for each site. We always conducted measurements and surveys at two different locations at the same time and compared the thermal situations between the two sites to evaluate the thermal effects of green, water, and shading. The thermal effect was assessed by considering the differences in the average air temperature and physiological equivalent temperature (PET) over the entire measurement period, which is mostly from noon until the late afternoon, and the difference in the thermal experience of the respondents.

Table 1 Date of measurements, maximum air temperature measured at Schiphol airport (Ta,max), name and type of location, situation with respect to sun or shade and description of the measured urban spaces, including tree species and dimensions of the water bodies and the Local Climate Zone (Stewart and Oke 2012). The colour indicates the type of location: grey are impervious urban spaces, green are parks with grass or shrubs and blue are spaces near urban water. It is indicated if measurements were done in the sun (yellow), in the shade of trees (green) or in the shade of buildings (red). Also listed is the measured time period, the number of interviews in shade and sun, the average meteorological conditions—air temperature (Ta) and physiological equivalent temperature (PET) in the shade and in the sun, relative humidity (RH) and wind speed (u). *Measurements done at Oosterdok are not used in this paper

During some days in the first measurement year 2015, we moved the weather station each hour between a sunlit and a shaded location (see Table 1). By doing so, we could in addition assess the thermal difference between a sunlit and a shaded area for one type of location, apart from the thermal difference between two types of locations. For example, from the measurements at the locations Leidseplein (impervious location E) and Vondelpark (green location O) on 30-6-2015 (Table 1), we could estimate the thermal effect of a green urban space (Vondelpark) with respect to an impervious urban location (Leidseplein) for sunny as well as shaded conditions. In addition, we could assess the shading effect of trees for a green space (i.e. Vondelpark), and the shading effect of trees for an impervious urban space (i.e. Leideplein). However, moving a weather station each hour between two locations also turned out to complicate the analyses considerably. Therefore, in the second season (2016), we measured with each mobile weather station at only one location throughout the day.

Measurement days and meteorological conditions

The dates in Table 1 show that measurements were carried out on 12 different days in the summer months (June, July and August) of 2015 and 2016. All days were weekdays. The measurements were taken during the hottest part of the day, starting from around noon until late afternoon. In 2015, four measurement days were part of a heat wave period in the Netherlands lasting from 30th of June to 5th of July 2015. A heatwave period in the Netherlands is defined by the Netherlands Royal Meteorological Institute as a period of five consecutive days with a maximum temperature above 25 °C including at least three on which the maximum temperature exceeds 30 °C. Days for which air temperatures above 25 °C and clear skies were forecasted, were chosen to carry out the measurements and the surveys. For this reason, most air temperatures measured at the urban sites reached maximum temperatures well above 25 °C. However, on some days, air temperatures remained lower than forecasted (e.g. 26 August 2016), but PET values always exceeded 33 °C in the sun, indicating a situation of at least moderate heat stress (Matzarakis and Mayer 1996). Table 1 also lists the maximum air temperature measured at Schiphol airport, a rural reference station of the Netherlands Royal Meteorological Institute 10 km from the centre of Amsterdam. Also at this site, air temperatures did not always exceed 25 °C.

The meteorological measurements listed in Table 1 indicate that the average humidity on the measurement days was low, between 33 and 68%, and wind speeds measured were calm up to light breeze. These values reflect a typical situation for an urban environment on a hot summer day.

Micrometeorological measurements and assessment of PET

To measure the meteorological conditions and estimate the PET, we equipped two-wheel hand carts with a Davis Vantage Pro2 weather station (see Fig. 2). This instrument includes sensors that measure air temperature, air humidity, wind speed, wind direction, and global radiation and uses a fan-aspirated radiation shield. The accuracy of the sensors are 0.5 °C for air temperature, 3% for air humidity, 5% for wind speed, 3° for wind direction and 5% for global radiation. The globe temperature was measured using an additional 38-mm flat grey globe thermometer, made of a grey painted table tennis ball using a flat grey paint (RAL 7001) following Thorsson et al. (2007). A temperature probe of Davis (type 6372) was used inside the table tennis ball. The accuracy of this sensor is 0.5 °C. The time interval of the recorded meteorological variables was 1 min. The height at which the sensors were installed is 1.1 m, corresponding to the average height of an adult’s centre of gravity (Mayer and Hoeppe 1987).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mobile weather station. Davis Vantage Pro2 instrument installed on a wheel hand cart

From the globe temperature, the wind speed and the air temperature, the mean radiant temperature (Tmrt; Mayer and Hoeppe 1987) could be obtained using the method as suggested by Thorsson et al. (2007):

$$ {T}_{\mathrm{mrt}}={\left[{\left({T}_{\mathrm{g}}+273.15\right)}^4+\frac{1.335\times {10}^8{u}^6}{\varepsilon {D}^4}\times \left({T}_{\mathrm{g}}-{T}_{\mathrm{a}}\right)\right]}^{\raisebox{1ex}{$1$}\!\left/ \!\raisebox{-1ex}{$4$}\right.}-273.15 $$
(1)

where Tg is the globe temperature (°C), u is wind speed (m/s), Ta is air temperature (°C), ε is the globe emissivity (0.95) and D is the globe diameter (m).

Tmrt is the most important variable in assessing the thermal comfort situation and calculating the PET. It sums the human body exposure to all short- and long-wave radiation fluxes (direct, diffuse, reflected and emitted) in a given environment (Johansson et al. 2014). Since the method used to assess Tmrt is very sensitive to variations in wind speed, we used 10-min running averages of the meteorological variables as input for calculating Tmrt.

The PET was calculated using Rayman software (Matzarakis et al. 2007). Air temperature, relative humidity, wind speed and Tmrt were used as input and the default settings of Rayman were used: gender—male, age—35 years of age, weight —75 kg, length—1.75 m, standing, moderate clothing (clo = 0.9) and doing light work (80.0 W).

Field surveys

In order to gain insight into the perception of the thermal environment of the urban spaces, we conducted interviews and surveyed people in the near environment of the weather stations, ensuring that the interviewees were in the same thermal environment as the measurement devices. We questioned people walking or sitting in this area. A structured interview form was prepared for the interviews (Table 2). People were asked to give some personal information related to the individual factors that determine thermal sensation (part 2 of the form) and their opinion on the thermal environment (part 3 of the form). Note that some slight differences exist between the surveys used in 2015 and 2016. We based the questions of the surveys on studies of Johansson et al. (2014), Klemm et al. (2015a), Lenzholzer and Van der Wulp (2010) and Nikolopoulou and Lykoudis (2006). Filling in a questionnaire took 2 to 3 min and was supported by a researcher.

Table 2 Overview of the questions in the survey

The total number of surveys that were completed and could be used in this study is 1928. Table 1 lists the number of people that were interviewed at each location. The average gender division of the respondents is 55% male, 42% female and of 3% unknown. The personal characteristics of the respondents with respect to age and origin are presented in Tables 3 and 4. Since most of the measured urban spaces are situated in touristic areas, the number of respondents from countries other than the Netherlands is relatively high (nearly 50%).

Table 3 Age of the respondents
Table 4 Origin of the respondents

The dataset that we created by doing this questionnaire survey is extensive and can be used to answer a wide range of research questions on thermal experience and its relationship with individual factors such as clothing, age and cultural background. In this study, we especially focused on the answers to the first 4 questions concerning thermal experience (questions 3a, b, c and d). We also checked for some relationships with individual factors. Chi-square tests performed on the dataset of 2015 showed however that experienced thermal comfort (question 3b: Do you find this environment thermally comfortable/slightly uncomfortable/uncomfortable/very uncomfortable) does not have a significant (p < 0.05) relationship with any of the individual factors age, gender, clothing, activity level, cultural background or thermal history. In other words, the influence of individual factors on how people experience and value the thermal conditions of the urban spaces was not found in the dataset.

Methodology to assess the thermal effect

The key results of this study are the thermal effects of green, water and shading in urban spaces. The thermal effects are evaluated in terms of differences in air temperature, PET and thermal experience.

Table 5 shows which locations (L1 and L2) were compared with each other to estimate the thermal effects. Each time L1 is the expected cooler situation (green, water, shade) and L2 is the reference location. First, the thermal effects of water and urban green (grass and shrubs in parks) were assessed. This was done separately for shaded and sunlit locations. Second, the thermal effects of shading by buildings and shading by trees were assessed. The effect of shading by buildings could be assessed for impervious urban spaces only, while the effect of shading by trees could be assessed for two situations: trees in green (parks) and trees in impervious urban spaces. Note that each time only measurements from the same day were compared.

Table 5 Thermal effects of water, green (grass and shrubs) and shading measured as the difference in air temperature, PET and thermal experience between two locations (L1 and L2). The colours of L1 and L2 indicate the type of location: impervious (grey), grass or shrubs (green) and near urban water (blue). n.s. means that the difference in temperature or thermal comfort is not significant (p = 0.05). Bold numbers indicate that the location L2 is cooler—which is counterintuitive. The yellow and dark grey cells for thermal perception, comfort, preference and acceptation indicate a significant difference in thermal experience between L1 and L2. The significance level (p value) is given. Yellow indicates that L1 is perceived cooler, more comfortable or acceptable. Grey indicates that L2 is perceived cooler—which is counterintuitive. Concerning preference, yellow indicates that people at L2 prefer a cooler environment than at L1. The thermal effects are numbered from 1 to 19 in line with Fig. 3.

The thermal effects of water and green urban spaces are always based on measurements and surveys at two locations with two weather stations at the same time. The differences in air temperature and PET listed in Table 5 were therefore calculated as the average of the differences between the two time series (1-min time intervals). A paired sample t-test was used to determine whether the mean difference between the two series is significant (p < 0.05). The thermal effects of shaded locations are mostly based on measurements from a weather station that was moved each hour between a sunlit and a shaded location. The differences in air temperature and PET listed in Table 5 were therefore calculated as the difference in the average air temperature and PET measured at the two locations. An unpaired sample t-test was used to determine whether this difference is significant (p < 0.05).

For assessing the differences in thermal experience, we compared interviewed thermal perception, thermal comfort, thermal preference and thermal acceptation between the locations. This corresponds to the four questions of the survey:

  1. 3a.

    Thermal perception—How are you feeling now? cold/cool/slightly cool/neutral/slightly warm/warm/hot

  2. 3b.

    Thermal comfort—Do you find this environment thermally: comfortable/slightly uncomfortable/uncomfortable/very uncomfortable

  3. 3c.

    Thermal preference—How would you prefer it to be now: cooler/no change/warmer

  4. 3d.

    Thermal acceptation—On thermal level, this environment is for me: acceptable/unacceptable

A chi-square test of independence was used to test the difference in perception, comfort, preference and acceptation between two locations. This test is applicable, since we compare two categorical data sets: thermal experience versus the type of urban space. Kendall’s Tau-test was applied for thermal acceptation when the sample size of the group ‘unacceptable’ was too small for applying a chi-square test. Chi-square examines the difference in distribution of two groups and Kendall’s tau-test examines the difference in ranking of the two groups.

Results

Figure 3 and Table 5 show the effects of green (grass and shrubs), water and shading for air temperature, PET and thermal experience. The results clearly demonstrate that the thermal effect of shading is largest. This is especially reflected by large differences in PET of more than 10 °C between sunlit and shaded locations during the hot summer afternoon. The effect of water and green (grass and shrubs) in urban spaces on the PET ranges up to 4 °C only. Another clear result depicted in Fig. 3 is that water, green and shading have little impact on the air temperature and reduce the average air temperature by roughly 1 °C, which is close to the measurement accuracy of the sensor (±0.7 °C). The surveys (Table 5) especially indicate significant effects of shading on thermal perception and thermal comfort, but generally no significant effects for water and green.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The measured thermal effect (expressed as cooling effect) of water, green (grass and shrubs) and shading by buildings and trees on the air temperature (dark grey) and PET (light grey). The numbers on the x-axis link to the numbers of the thermal effects reported in Table 5

In more detail, the measurements presented in Fig. 3 indicate that the cooling effect of water with respect to air temperature is very small (less than 1 °C). For one situation (no. 2, i.e. the effect of Amstel river), the thermal effect is even slightly negative, meaning that the air temperature near Amstel river was higher than at the reference location. This might be caused by the high buildings causing more fresh airflow at the reference location (L2). These small temperature differences, however, can also be explained by the sensor accuracy.

PET values generally show a small cooling effect of urban water bodies, ranging up to 4 °C for the Amstel river (effect no. 3), while for four situations higher PET values were measured near the water bodies. The reason for higher PET values near urban water bodies is different for each location and can often be explained by a higher humidity, less wind or a higher Tmrt at the urban space near the water body with respect to the reference location L2. For instance, the wind speed on 25 August 2016 at the IJ promenade was much larger than at the reference location (Table 1), a difference of 40%. By using the relationship expressed by Eq. 1, this resulted in a higher estimate of Tmrt and a higher PET at the water body than at the reference location. This explains the outlier in Table 5 and Fig. 3 (no. 7).

The small thermal effect of water bodies is also reflected by the surveys. Thermal perception, comfort, preference and acceptation were not significantly different at the locations near water bodies. At one location (fountain on Frederiksplein; no. 1 in Table 5), the respondents perceived the environment near the fountain even warmer than some meters away from it (dark grey cell in Table 5). This we cannot explain from the measurements. It might be related to the fact that Frederiksplein was the only situation where the thermal effect of water was measured for shaded conditions. Also air temperatures were relatively low for this day, about 23 °C (Table 1). Both conditions do not favour thermal differences between urban spaces.

Also the cooling effect of green urban spaces (grass and shrubs) was found to be small in the measurements, ranging up to 1 °C in air temperature and 4 °C in PET. In addition, there is no clear difference in the thermal effect of grass and shrubs between measurements carried out in shade or sun (nos. 8 and 9 in Table 5 and Fig. 3). Neither did people perceive the thermal situation of the green urban space different from the impervious urban spaces, except for one situation, i.e. Vondelpark (no. 9 in Table 5 and Fig. 3). For this situation, urban green (Vondelpark) was rated significantly more comfortable than the impervious urban space Leidseplein, while at the same time the respondents perceived Vondelpark significantly warmer than Leidseplein. These results demonstrate that urban spaces that are perceived as more comfortable (Vondelpark), are not necessarily the same as the locations that are perceived as cooler, even on hot days.

The measurements particularly show that shading by buildings or trees has the largest cooling effect on the urban environment, without clear difference in shading by buildings or trees. This is reflected by large differences in PET shown in Table 5 and Fig. 3. The cooling effect of shading ranges between 12 and 22 °C in PET, depending on the situation. The measured cooling effect of shading in terms of air temperature is small and ranges up to 1 °C. The surveys indicate that at 4 out of 9 sets of locations, shaded spaces were rated as cooler and at two sets, shaded locations were evaluated as more comfortable than sunlit locations. Also, thermal preference was significantly different between four sets of sunlit and shaded locations, which means that less people preferred a cooler space when they were in the shade.

We also grouped the survey results into four datasets (i.e. water, green, shading by buildings, shading by trees, following Table 5) and investigated the effect in thermal experience for each of these groups. These findings first of all demonstrate that the respondents usually rated the thermal situation of the urban spaces as slightly warm to hot (Fig. 4a). Still, most urban spaces are regarded as thermally comfortable (Fig. 4b), which again implies that slightly warm to hot is not directly linked to uncomfortable. There is a preference at all locations towards cooler urban spaces instead of warmer spaces, but most people prefer no change (Fig. 4c). Almost all respondents assess the investigated urban spaces as thermally acceptable (Fig. 4d).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Survey results showing the effect of water, green, shading by buildings and shading by trees on thermal perception (a), thermal comfort (b), thermal preference (c) and thermal acceptation (d). The thermal effects are indicated by presenting the respondent rates when data are grouped for L1 and L2 locations according to Table 5 (water: nos. 1–7, green: nos. 8–10, shading by buildings: nos. 11–15, and shading by trees: nos. 16–19). The effect of shading by buildings on the thermal perception (a) and thermal comfort (b) are statistically significant (p < 0.05)

The results in Fig. 4 also indicate that the distribution in thermal experience found at the types of urban spaces that are expected to be cooler (water, green or shading) does not differ much from the reference situation (impervious or sunlit): The surveyed thermal experience of urban spaces close to water bodies is not significantly different from the results at impervious urban spaces. Likewise, the thermal experience of green urban spaces (grass and shrubs) is not different from the impervious sites. Shading by buildings and trees causes the largest effects in thermal experience: Shaded spaces are more often rated as cold, cool or neutral, and more comfortable than sunlit spaces. However, only locations shaded by buildings create statistically different responses in thermal perception (Fig. 4a; p = 0.000) and thermal comfort (Fig. 4b; p = 0.0267). All other effects of water, green and shading on the thermal experience were statistically insignificant.

Discussion

On the results compared to earlier studies

The thermal effect of water, green and shading of buildings and trees found in this study are in line with earlier research. The large thermal effect of shading is for instance in agreement with findings of Middel et al. (2016) who investigated the thermal effect of shaded spaces by trees and artificial shading canopies (photovoltaic canopies) by using meteorological observations and field surveys in a hot desert city in Arizona. They concluded that shade plays an important role in designing pedestrian-friendly spaces in cities and that artificial and natural shades are equally efficient. Our results also indicate that shading has a major impact in improving the thermal comfort, in which trees (natural shades) are equally efficient as buildings (artificial shade), especially in terms of PET. This is also in line with Chan et al. (2017), who found a positive correlation between people’s thermal comfort and the perceived amount of trees and shade provision in urban parks in Tokyo. They conclude that thermal comfort in parks on summer days could be improved by placing more trees and shade provisions.

The cooling potential of trees was also demonstrated by Klemm et al. (2015a) who measured a substantial improvement of the physical thermal conditions in the Netherlands through the shading effect of street greenery (trees). These meteorological measurements indicated that streets with trees reduce Tmrt by up to 5 °C. The results of Klemm et al. (2015a) also revealed that trees have little influence on the air temperature. This is consistent with our results demonstrating a small thermal effect of trees on air temperature (about 1 °C cooling) and a considerable effect on PET (ranging up to 19 °C cooling). Large reductions in PET up to 16 °C were also found by de Abreu-Harbich et al. (2015) for several tree species in a tropical climate. Their effect depends on the size and the shape of the tree crown, the shape of the leaves, the trunk, the permeability of the crown and the clustering of trees. This was also confirmed by Kong et al. (2017) who investigated how various trees impact thermal comfort in Tokyo. They therefore suggest to plant appropriate vegetation (with respect to tree species and tree formation) for effective tree shading and improvement of the thermal conditions. We however did not differentiate between tree species and clustering in our analysis, since at most urban spaces with trees, urban trees were large and plenty. Moreover, such study would require another measurement set-up and more accurate measurement equipment that can measure smaller temperature differences.

Mobile meteorological measurements carried out with cargo bikes in Rotterdam, the Netherlands, during hot summer days (Heusinkveld et al. 2014; Heusinkveld et al. 2011) also illustrated the large effect of shading on the PET. They measured differences in PET due to effects of shading exceeding 15 °C. They therefore recommend to take shading into consideration in the layout of streets.

In addition, the study of Heusinkveld et al. (2011) demonstrated that the cooling effect of urban water is ambiguous and in the order of 1 °C in air temperature during daytime, which is similar to our findings. Model simulations of Theeuwes et al. (2013) revealed a similar decrease of only 1 °C in air temperature and 0.5 °C in WBGT (wet bulb globe temperature) close to an urban lake, which supports our conclusion that the cooling effect of small water bodies in cities is likely to be small. Saaroni and Ziv (2003) demonstrated that the thermal effect of urban water bodies also depends on wind speed and direction, apart from the size of the water body. Generally, the cooling effect at the downwind side of the pond is larger than at the upwind side. Since all our measurements were carried out at the downwind side of the water bodies, we could not differentiate and investigate the upwind or downwind cooling effect in detail. Neither, we could investigate the differences in the thermal effect between the several types and sizes of water bodies (e.g. urban ponds, rivers and lakes), since each water body was measured on a different summer day. Such study would again require another measurement set-up and more accurate instruments.

On the methodology: the field survey

Whereas the meteorological measurements in this study generally indicate a statistically significant thermal effect of urban green, water and shading in air temperature and PET, the results of the survey questionnaires did not show much difference in the thermal experience between the urban spaces. The results of the field surveys seem therefore somehow inconsistent to the meteorological observations, which can be explained in several ways. First of all, urban spaces which are measured as cooler do not necessarily need to be perceived as cooler. Second, the contradictions between the measured and experienced thermal effects can be due to inaccuracies in the measurements or limitations of the methodology. In our case, the characteristics of the group of respondents might have influenced towards more positive and thermally comfortable outcomes, because:

  • The group of respondents might have been biased by too many tourists, since the measurements were often carried out at touristic spots. This might have resulted in a more positive reaction towards the warm thermal situation of the urban spaces, since most of the tourists are likely to be happy and feeling comfortable on holidays. This, however, was not reflected in the results as people from outside the Netherlands (if we can regard this group as the tourists) did not evaluate the thermal comfort situation of the urban spaces different from people living in the Netherlands. Moreover, a statistical analysis indicated that the country of origin did not have a significant relationship with thermal comfort (p = 0.88) in our dataset; a linkage that was for instance found by Kenawy and Elkadi (2013). On the other hand, it might also be justified to evaluate the thermal situation of touristic spots by tourists since they are the actual users of these areas.

  • In addition, the group of respondents might have been biased by people that enjoy the thermal situation of the hot summer day. The hot weather situation might have forced people that do not like the high temperatures to stay in other (cooler) urban areas than the ones that were measured. Vulnerable people might even have stayed at home. As a consequence, people that would have rated the selected urban spaces as uncomfortable and unacceptable might be missing from the group of respondents, because they chose to stay in other places. This hypothesis is supported by Rutty and Scott (2015), who studied visitors’ thermal comfort in the Caribbean beaches. They found that beach users’ thermal preference in UTCI (Universal Thermal Climate Index) is up to 18 °C warmer than the preferred thermal condition identified in existing outdoor bioclimatic studies from urban park settings. This bias can be regarded as a shortcoming of the research methodology and would for instance be an argument for using thermal walks (e.g. Lenzholzer et al. 2016), in which a selected group of respondents assesses the thermal situation of several urban spaces. At the same time, it might not be necessary to include all types of citizens, but only those that make use of the selected urban spaces during a hot day and who were actually part of the survey.

On the comparison between measurements and surveys

As indicated in the previous section, the micrometeorological measurements and surveys show some remarkable differences. In contrast to the measurements, the questionnaire surveys showed no statistically significant thermal effects of urban green (grass and shrubs) and urban water. According to the surveys, only the thermal effect of shading by buildings was statistically significant in terms of being cooler and more comfortable. Similarly, Klemm et al. (2015a) found a statistically significant thermal effect of street greenery on air temperature and Tmrt, but did not find a statistically significant relationship between perceived thermal comfort and the amount of street greenery.

In the introduction of this paper, we also raised the questions: Do meteorological measurements and questionnaire surveys provide the same insight and do surveys add relevant information to the measurements in advising urban professionals and planners? We noticed that meteorological measurements especially give information about the size of the thermal effect, leading to numbers and measurable quantities. This can be of support for urban professionals to decide on the size of interventions and the target value for a thermally comfortable urban design. In the near future, this will become more important in the Netherlands as municipalities might need to demonstrate that their urban areas are heat resilient or well enough adapted to heat. To monitor or assess the thermal situation or the effectiveness of specific heat adaptation measures in an urban space, measurements seem to be a more appropriate approach than using interview surveys.

On the other hand, results from interview surveys provide essential insights in the thermal perception of urban spaces, since it is important that urban spaces which are designed to be cool and comfortable are also valued and perceived by people as cool and comfortable. This information can also be meaningful in convincing urban planners to choose for a certain design. Additionally, interview surveys can provide valuable information on the psychological factors that play a role in the thermal perception, such as mood (question 2c in Table 2), the time of exposure (question 2g in Table 2), expectations or naturalness (Chan et al. 2017; Nikolopoulou and Steemers 2003). These psychological factors were not investigated in this study, but are still valuable when designing thermally comfortable urban spaces. Both thermal perception and the psychological aspects of urban designs can only be investigated by qualitative methods such as observing people’s behavior or conducting interview surveys (Lenzholzer et al. 2016) and cannot be retrieved from meteorological measurements.

On the implications for urban professionals

The major aim of this study was to answer the question of urban professionals which urban spaces provide substantial cooling and are most comfortable during warm days. We aimed to answer this question by using meteorological observations and field surveys. Based on these results, we strongly advise urban professionals to consider shading in designing thermally comfortable urban spaces by using either artificial or natural shades. The shading of trees might be favourable above artificial shading elements because of the many co-benefits of urban trees (Mullaney et al. 2015), including positive effects on mental and physical health, biodiversity and real estate values. Looking at the year round situation in the Netherlands, deciduous trees, which are leafless in winter, are preferable when radiation of the sun is most welcome. When designing outdoor shade, it is also important to consider location, orientation, time and day (Lin et al. 2010) to provide thermally comfortable conditions throughout the entire year and for all users. To allow users to choose their preferred thermal comfort level, Lin et al. (2010) also recommend to use multiple shading types and different shading levels.

We concluded that the cooling potential of small water bodies in cities is negligible and therefore not a good adaptation measure to create cool urban spaces. In discussions with urban professionals from municipalities, we discovered that these conclusions are not easily accepted, since water in urban areas is generally believed to have a cooling effect. Still, other types of urban water might have a more efficient cooling potential, such as water sprays, tangible water, or a combination of water and shading from trees (e.g. Coutts et al. 2013; Nishimura et al. 1998). Urban designers should therefore carefully weigh the use of urban water to improve the pedestrian thermal comfort and might need to consider other forms of water.

A topic that was not investigated in this study but which is important for urban professionals to adapt cities to heat are the design requirements to be sufficiently adapted to urban heat. Principles to guide climate-responsive design and planning with respect to heat are still lacking (Bowler et al. 2010). In addition, there is little understanding of what people and citizens prefer or need. Sometimes a PET of > 41 °C, indicating extreme heat stress in Western/Central Europe (Matzarakis and Mayer 1996; Cheung and Jim 2017), is used as a threshold value for designing. (NB: Table 1 shows that this PET-value was exceeded on 18 of the 21 locations measured in the sun.) Investigating the thermal perception, comfort and preferences with field surveys could help in setting these design requirements and making choices which urban spaces are experienced as thermally comfortable.

Conclusions

  • The findings of this research prove that shading urban spaces (by trees or buildings) is the most effective in creating cooler conditions in urban environments during hot summer days. This is confirmed by PET values which are 12 to 22 °C lower than sunlit spaces. Urban green spaces (grass and shrubs) were also found to be cooler in terms of PET, but their cooling effect is much smaller (2 to 4 °C reduction in PET). Also, the thermal effect of small urban water bodies is small and ambiguous, since it sometimes seems to cool and sometimes seems to warm the near urban environment (between − 4 °C and 5 °C PET).

  • The air temperature of urban spaces is not much influenced through shading, urban water or grass. The thermal effect of these types of urban spaces on air temperature is around 1 °C cooling.

  • The results of this study highlight the importance of shading in urban design to reduce heat stress. We therefore strongly advise urban professionals to consider shading in designing thermally comfortable urban spaces.

  • Meteorological measurements are especially useful to understand the physical thermal conditions and effects of urban spaces and designs. Since meteorological data provide quantifiable thermal effects, they can be more easily linked and translated to target values or guidelines to reduce urban heat in practice than surveys.

  • Surveys provide insights in the thermal perception of an urban design and the psychological factors involved. This type of information is especially interesting to set design requirements for urban spaces to be thermally comfortable.