Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of a coding intervention on what teachers learn to notice during whole-group discussion

  • Published:
Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article examines how a coding scheme for mathematics classroom discussion that was created to highlight how teachers negotiate student responses during whole-class discussion around high-level, cognitively demanding tasks was used to help teachers shift what they notice when analyzing classroom discourse. Data from an intervention that trained teachers how to use the coding scheme and then provided them opportunities to use the scheme to code transcripts of classroom discussion are presented. Results suggest that teachers’ ability to notice interactions between teacher and students when analyzing classroom discussion (as opposed to focusing on one actor or the other) can be increased and that teachers can learn to identify specific discourse moves teachers use to negotiate student responses. However, teachers’ capacity to identify how students’ opportunities to learn are related to teacher discourse moves did not change as a result of the intervention. The article goes on to examine how discussion during the intervention itself may have contributed to what teachers learned to notice. This research contributes to the body of work on teachers’ noticing by examining the feasibility and efficacy of using transcripts and a coding scheme to foster teachers’ ability to notice how they can increase their students’ opportunities to learn through mathematics discourse.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The coding scheme was developed at the University of Pittsburgh, Learning Research and Development Center by Kevin Ashley, Richard Correnti, Moddy McKeown, Peg Smith, Mary Kay Stein, James Chisholm, and Jimmy Scherrer.

  2. Power Analysis was conducted using G*Power 3.1 with the following parameters F test, ANOVA: repeated measures, within-between interaction with the following inputs: effect size = .25, alpha = .05, Power = .8, two groups, two measurements, correlation between measurements = .85.

  3. Following the advice of several researchers (Putnam and Borko 2000; Rodgers 2002), the intervention was designed to have teachers take an interpretive stance when coding transcripts rather than an evaluative stance. That is, the purpose of the coding activities was to determine what type of question a teacher was asking and how s/he used the student responses to that question; it was not to decide if the initial question and follow-up moves were “good” or “poor.” Although it is an open question that we have not tested, we hypothesize the evaluative stance would require a more “expert” coder.

  4. There were a few decision points that included only a participant uttering a code. Hence, a few decision points had a length of 1 line.

  5. Since the purpose of this intervention was to help participants understand that there are different moves that can be used to negotiate student responses (not to demonstrate that certain moves may be associated with student outcomes), some codes from the original ATM were collapsed. For example, the original ATM includes two codes named Pushback and Uptake. Both of these codes use student responses in an attempt to further the discussion. The only difference is that Pushback implicitly challenges the student to rethink her contribution. Participants in pilot studies had difficulty distinguishing the two. Although future empirical testing may indeed associate different student outcomes with use of these two codes, the primary investigator of this study felt that the laborious attempts of participants in this particular intervention to come to consensus on whether a move was either Pushback or Uptake would not be a productive use of time when attempting to move the group toward the goals of the intervention.

References

  • Ball, D. L. (2000). Bridging practices: Intertwining content and pedagogy in teaching and learning to teach. Journal of Teacher Education, 51(3), 241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, D. L. (2001). Teaching, with respect to mathematics and students. In T. Wood, B. Nelson & J. Warfield (Eds.), Beyond classical pedagogy: Teaching elementary school mathematics (pp. 11–22). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellack, A. A., Kliebard, H. M., Hyman, R. T., & Smith, F. L. (1966). The language of the classroom. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapin, S., O’Connor, C., & Anderson, N. (2003). Classroom discussions: Using math talk to help students learn. Sausalito, CA: Math Solutions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, L. (2007). Experimental methodology (10th ed.). Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dantonio, M., & Beisenherz, P. (2001). Learning to question, questioning to learn: Developing effective teacher questioning practices. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franke, M. L., Carpenter, T., Fennema, E., Ansell, E., & Behrend, J. (1998). Understanding teachers’ self-sustaining, generative change in the context of professional development. Teaching and Teacher Education, 14(1), 67–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. P., & Green, J. L. (1998). Discourse analysis, learning, and social practice: A methodological study. Review of Research in Education, 23(1), 119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, L. T., & Schifter, D. (1997). Understanding teachers in transition: Characteristics of a model for the development of mathematics teaching. In E. Fennema & B. Nelson (Eds.), Mathematics teachers in transition (pp. 19–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, E. (2007). Sensation and perception. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. K., & Morita, E. (2002). Japanese and American teachers’ evaluations of videotaped mathematics lessons. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 33(3), 154–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J., Yoshida, M., Stigler, J., & Fernandez, C. (1997). Japanese and American teachers’ evaluations of mathematics lessons: A new technique for exploring beliefs. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 16(1), 7–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lampert, M. (2001). Teaching problems and the problems of teaching. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, B. S. (1999). Building new knowledge by thinking: How administrators can learn what they need to know about mathematics education reform. Newton, MA: Education Development Center, Center for Development for Teaching.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palincsar, A. S. (1998). Social constructivist perspectives on teaching and learning. In J. Spence, J. Darley, & D. Foss (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (pp. 345–375). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, B., & Leatham, K. (2009). Learning to use students’ mathematical thinking to orchestrate a class discussion. In L. Knott (Ed.), The role of mathematics discourse in producing leaders of discourse (pp. 99–128). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, R. T., & Borko, H. (2000). What do new views of knowledge and thinking have to say about research on teacher learning? Educational Researcher, 29(1), 4–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodgers, C. (2002). Defining reflection: Another look at John Dewey and reflective thinking. The Teachers College Record, 104(4), 842–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Analyzing collaborative discourse. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 187–204). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1998). Toward a theory of teaching-in-context. Issues in Education, 4(1), 1–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherin, M., & van Es, E. (2005). Using video to support teachers’ ability to notice classroom interactions. Journal of Technology and Teacher Education, 13(3), 475–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M., & Stein, M. K. (2011). 5 practices for orchestrating productive mathematics discussions. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Star, J. R., & Strickland, S. K. (2008). Learning to observe: Using video to improve preservice mathematics teachers’ ability to notice. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 11(2), 107–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, M. K., Engle, R. A., Smith, M. S., & Hughes, E. K. (2008). Orchestrating productive mathematical discussions: Five practices for helping teachers move beyond show and tell. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10(4), 313–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, M. K., & Smith, M. S. (1998). Mathematical tasks as a framework for reflection: From research to practice. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 3(4), 268–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., & Hiebert, J. (1998). Teaching is a cultural activity. American Educator, 22(4), 4–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S., Rieke, R., & Janik, A. (1984). An introduction to reasoning. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Es, E., & Sherin, M. (2008). Mathematics teachers’ “learning to notice” in the context of a video club. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24, 244–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jimmy Scherrer.

Appendices

Appendix 1

ATMjrFootnote 5

Moves that begin a discussion

launch: open-ended question that invites student thinking

reinitiate: repeating or rephrasing a question that has already been asked

Moves that elaborate or deepen students’ knowledge by furthering the discussion

uptake: an open-ended question that uses a student response to extend or deepen the discussion

connection: linking ideas by comparing responses, solutions, or representations

Moves that elicit information

literal: a bounded question (i.e., not open-ended)

collecting: gathering additional responses to a question

Other moves

provides information: giving or telling information (including hints) to students

think aloud: modeling a thought process

lot: acknowledging that a response will be discussed later

terminal: ignoring or discontinuing a response

repeat: echoing a student response

NC; turn cannot be categorized using one of the codes above

Appendix 2

The illustration makes salient the conceptual space that could represent the territory of a fourth-grade discussion on quadrilaterals. Although the graphic itself would not be displayed, the example Launch below could be used to start a discussion that might eventually lead to the student creation of such an illustration.

Example: The teacher draws the following shape on the board

See Table 3.

Table 3 Illustrating three different ways a teacher might handle the student response, “It is a quadrilateral”

Appendix 3

See Table 4.

Table 4 Quantitative results of pre- and post-test

Appendix 4

General coding rules for ATM

  • When assigning multiple codes to a single turn, assign codes in the order in which the words appear in the transcripts.

  • Code according to the transcript (not according to what you think happened in the class).

  • Do not apply more than one code to the same set of words.

  • When assigning codes, you need to take into account the previous student’s response. You may take into account other students’ responses/contributions that have been up to that point in the lesson.

  • Do not consider any responses that follow a teacher turn in order to code the teacher turn.

  • Use the most specific code as possible when coding (e.g., code for uptake instead of launch when an open-ended question is asked based on a student contribution).

  • Clarifying questions such as, “Does everybody understand?” should be coded as literal.

  • Only code “NC” if no other codes are applicable to the turn.

  • Code “Okay,” “Ah ha,” or other habits of speech as NC if it constitutes the entire turn.

Appendix 5

Pre-and post-test questions

  1. 1.

    What did you pay attention to while watching this piece of discussion?

  2. 2.

    What else did you notice during the discussion?

  3. 3.

    How would you rate this piece of discussion on a 1–10 scale, with 1 reflecting a low-level discussion and a 10 reflecting a high-level discussion?

  4. 4.

    What about this discussion made you give it a score of ______?

  5. 5.

    What do you feel the teacher did well?

  6. 6.

    What are some ways the teacher could have altered students’ opportunities to learn?

  7. 7.

    How might the teacher have been able to improve the discussion?

  8. 8.

    What are some explicit moves you saw the teacher do?

  9. 9.

    Additional thoughts about the discussion.

  10. 10.

    What question would you ask next if you were the teacher? Why?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Scherrer, J., Stein, M.K. Effects of a coding intervention on what teachers learn to notice during whole-group discussion. J Math Teacher Educ 16, 105–124 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10857-012-9207-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10857-012-9207-2

Keywords

Navigation