Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Culture, neurobiology, and human behavior: new perspectives in anthropology

  • Review Essay
  • Published:
Biology & Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Our primary goal in this article is to discuss the cross-talk between biological and cultural factors that become manifested in the individual brain development, neural wiring, neurochemical homeostasis, and behavior. We will show that behavioral propensities are the product of both cultural and biological factors and an understanding of these interactive processes can provide deep insights into why people behave the way they do. This interdisciplinary perspective is offered in an effort to generate dialog and empirical work among scholars interested in merging aspects of anthropology and neuroscience, and anticipates that biological and cultural anthropology converge. We discuss new theoretical developments, hypothesis-testing strategies, and cross-disciplinary methods of observation and data collection. We believe that the exigency of integrating anthropology and the neurosciences is indisputable and anthropology’s role in an emerging interdisciplinary science of human behavior will be critical because its focus is, and has always been, on human biological and cultural systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. fMRI measures brain activity by investigating changes associated with the blood flow in a region of neurons. Given the average density of neurons and synapses in the cerebral cortex of about 12 × 104 and 9 × 108 per mm3, respectively, each voxel (the pixel of fMRI screens), captures blood flow in the region of approximately 80,000 neurons and more than 4 million synapses averaged over one second in this region. Thus, it is clear that the fMRI signal can just serve as an index of the overall activity of many neurons and processes (Raz 2012).

  2. External factors include also epigenetic information. In rhesus monkeys one-fifth of the entire genome is differentially methylated in brain cells compared to blood cells. This large epigenetic difference seems to be a function of early social experience (Suomi 2009). There is good reason to assume that the human genome undergoes epigenetic changes to a similar or greater extent due to extensive geneenvironment interactions.

  3. The terms “race” and “race bias” are used in much of the contemporary literature dealing with this subject. Anthropologists have rightfully rejected the traditional concept of “race” and the cited authors reject the traditional term as well; they clearly understand that “race” is a cultural construct.

  4. It should, however, be noted that unequivocal evidence is still missing whether oxytocin can actually cross the blood–brain barriere. Thus, most studies using nasal oxytocin sprays only provide indirect evidence of oxytocin effects on the brain as cerebrospinal fluids are usually not measured.

  5. However, one needs to take the hemodynamic response of fMRI studies into account that is about 4–6 s. This requires a precise temporal planning of the multi-paradigm set-up, something that is done for many other studies in which reaction time of subjects is an important parameter that must be controlled for.

References

  • Atran S, Navarro A, Ochsner K, Tobena A, Vilarroya O (2009) Values, empathy, and fairness across social barriers. New York Academy of Sciences, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar M (2011) Predictions in the brain: using our past to generate the future. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bickle J (2009) The Oxford handbook of philosophy and neuroscience. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boden M (2006) Mind as machine: a history of cognitive science. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bos PA, Terburg D, van Honk J (2010) Testosterone decreases trust in social naive humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107:9991–9995

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S (2004) Microeconomics: behavior, institutions, and evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S, Gintis H (2011) A cooperative species: human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza R, Kingstone A (2008) Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo JT, Bertson G (2002) Foundations in social neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer CF (2009) Behavioral game theory and the neural basis of strategic choice. In: Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (eds) Neuroeconomics decision making and the brain. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter S (2012) Psychology’s bold initiative. Science 335:1558–1561

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalupa LM, Berardi N, Caleo M, Galli-Resta L, Pizzorusso T (2011) Cerebral plasticity: new perspectives. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Changeux J-P (2004) The physiology of truth: neuroscience and human knowledge. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Charrier C, Joshi K, Coutinho-Budd J, Kim JE, Lambert N, de Marchena J, Jin WL, Vanderhaeghen P, Ghosh A, Sassa T, Polleux F (2012) Inhibition of SRGAP2 function by its human-specific paralogs induces neoteny during spine maturation. Cell 149:923–935

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao JY (2009) Cultural neuroscience: cultural influences on brain function, progress in brain research, vol 178. Elsevier, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiao JY, Bebko GM (2011) Cultural neuroscience of social cognition. In: Han S, Poppel E (eds) Culture and neural frames of cognition and communication. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiao JY, Blizinsky KD (2010) Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 277:529–537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland P (2007) Neurophilosophy at work. Cambridge University Press, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PS (2011) Braintrust: what neuroscience tells us about morality. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PS, Sejnowski TJ (1992) The computational brain. MIT Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloutier J, Li T, Correll J (2014) The impact of childhood experience on amygdala response to perceptually familiar black and white faces. J Cogn Neurosci 26:1992–2004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa-Mattioli M, Gobert D, Harding H, Herdy B, Azzi M, Bruno M, Bidinosti M et al (2005) Translational control of hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory by the eIF2alpha kinase GCN2. Nature 436:1166–1173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji MR (2004) Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychol Sci 15:806–813

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio A (2010) Self comes to mind: constructing the conscious brain. Pantheon Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu CKW, Greer LL, Handgraaf MJJ, Shalvin S, Van Kleef GA, Baas M, Ten Velden FS, Van Dijk E, Feith SWW (2010) The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328:1408–1411

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Haan M, Gunnar MR (2009) Handbook of developmental social neuroscience. Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • de Quervain DJ-F, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, Schellhammer M, Schnyder U, Buck A, Fehr E (2004) The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 305:1254–1258

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal F (2009) The age of empathy: nature’s lessons for a kinder society. Harmony Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (1997) The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (2003a) Multilevel selection in a complex adaptive system. The problem of language origins. In: Weber BH, Deprew DJ (eds) Evolution and learning: the Baldwin effect reconsidered. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 81–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (2003b) The hierarchic logic of emergence: untangling the interdependence of evolution and self-organization. In: Weber BH, Deprew DJ (eds) Evolution and learning: the Baldwin effect reconsidered. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 273–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (2012) Incomplete nature: how mind emerged from matter. WW Norton and Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Delgado MR, Locke HM, Stenger VA, Fiez JA (2003) Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: effects of valence and magnitute of manipulation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 3:27–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis MY, Nuttle X, Sudmant PH, Antonacci F, Graves TA, Nefedov M, Sajjadian S et al (2012) Evolution of human-species neural SRGAP2 genes by incomplete segmental duplication. Cell 149:912–922

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deoni SCL, Mercure E, Blasi A, Gasston D, Thomson A, Johnson M, Williams SCR, Murphy DGM (2011) Mapping infant brain myelination with magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 31:784–791

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doidge N (2007) The brain that changes itself. Penguin Books, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Donalson ZR, Young LJ (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322:900–904

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (2002) The social brain hypothesis. In: Cacioppo JT (ed) Foundations in social neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 69–88

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles JC (1989) Evolution of the brain: creation of the self. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman GM (1987) Neural Darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Einstein A, Infeld L (1938) The evolution of physics. Simon and Schuster, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk C, Zollikofer PE, Morimoto N, Ponce de Leon MS (2012) Metopic suture of Taung (Australopithecus africanus) and its implications for hominin evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109:8467–8470

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E (2009) Social preferences and the brain. In: Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (eds) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain. Academic Press, New York, pp 215–233

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U, Kosfeld M (2005) Neuroeconomic foundations of trust and social preferences. Centre for Economic Policy Research, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Flechsig PE (1901) Developmental (myelogenetic) localisation of the cerebral Cortexin human subject. Lancet ii:1027–1029

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith U, Firth C (2010) The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has gone. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365:165–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furman ME, Gallo FP (2000) The neurophysics of human behavior: explorations at the interface of brain, mind, behavior and information. CRC Press, Boca Raton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner H (1987) The mind’s new science: a history of cognitive revolution. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (2008) Human: the science behind what makes us unique. Ecco, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (2009) The cognitive neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary DC (2005) The origin of mind: evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. American Psychological Association, Washington

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis HM (2000) Game theory evolving: a problem-centered introduction to modeling strategic interaction. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis HM (2009) The bounds of reason: game theory and the unification of the behavioral sciences. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis HM, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (2005) Moral sentiments and material interests: the foundations of cooperation in economic life. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (2009) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray WD (2012) Introduction to volume 4 issue 3 topics. Top Cogn Sci 4:331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray CM, König P, Engel AK, Singer W (1989) Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338:334–337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guerrier S, Coutinho-Budd J, Sassa T, Gresset A, Jordan NV, Chen K, Jin W-L, Frost A, Polleux F (2009) The F-BAR domain of srGAP2 induces membrane protrusions required for neuronal migration and morphogenesis. Cell 138:990–1004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo S, Bao S (2010) srGAP2 arginine methylation regulates cell migration and cell spreading through promoting dimerization. J Biol Chem 285:35133–35141

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:4259–4264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han S, Poppel E (2011) Culture and neural frames of cognition and communication. Springer, Berlin

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haruno M, Frith CD (2010) Activity in the amygdala elicited by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nat Neurosci 13:160–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauert C, Traulsen A, Brandt H, Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2007) Via freedom to coercion: the emergence of costly punishment. Science 316:1905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauert C, Traulsen A, De Silva nee Brandt H (2008) Public goods with punishment and abstaining in finite and infinite populations. Biol Theory 3:114–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich JP, McElreath R (2007) Dual-inheritance theory: the evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In: Dunbar R, Barrett L (eds) Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 555–570

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich JP, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H (2004) Foundations of human sociality: economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM, Huber L (2000) The evolution of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull DL, Ruse M (1998) The philosophy of biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Immordino-Yang MH, McColl A, Darmasio H, Damasio A (2009) Neural correlates of admiration and compassion. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:8021–8026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E, Lamb MJ (2010) Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance. In: Pigliucci M, Muller GB (eds) Evolution—the extended synthesis. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 137–174

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jerison HJ (1975) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Curr Anthropol 16:403–426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaas JH (2013) The evolution of brains from early mammals to humans. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 4:33–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel ER, Squire LR (2000) Neuroscience: breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain and mind. Science 290:1113–1120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (2000) Principles of neural science. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A (2006) Ontogeny, genetics, and evolution: a perspective from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Biol Theory 1:44–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama S, Bowman NA (2011) Cultural consequences of voluntary settlement in the frontier: evidence and implications. In: Schaller ANM, Heine SJ, Yamagishi T, Kameda T (eds) Evolution, culture and the human mind. Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Knafo A, Israel S, Ebstein RP (2011) Heritability of children’s prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in dopamine receptors D4 gene. Dev Psychopathol 23:53–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson B, Delgado MR, Phillips PEM (2009) Representation of subjective value in the striatum. In: Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (eds) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch C (2004) The quest for consciousness: a neurobiological approach. Roberts and Company, Denver

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435:673–676

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kubota JT, Banaji MR, Phelps EA (2012) The neuroscience of race. Nat Neurosci 15:940–948

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Uller T, Feldman MW, Sterelny K, Müller GB, Moczek A, Jablonka E, Odling-Smee J (2015) The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. doi:10.1186/s12862-015-0448-4

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson DO (2006) Multidimensional selection and evolution: towards the scientific study of dual-inheritance, transmission of information, modularity, and complexity among humans. In: Paper presented at the program of theoretical and methodological issues in evolutionary archaeology: toward a unified paradigm. UISPP XVth Congress of Archaeology, Lisbon, Sept 4–9

  • Larson DO (2010) Innovations, replicative behavior and evolvability: contributions from the neurosciences and human making decision theory. In: O’Brien M, Shennan S (eds) Innovation in cultural systems: contributions from evolutionary anthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 69–80

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux JE (1996) The emotional brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon & Schuster, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux JE (2002) Synaptic self: how our brains become who we are. Viking, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lende DH, Downey G (2012) The encultured brain: an introduction to neuroanthropology. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Lillard A (1998) Ethnopsychologies: cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychol Bull 123:3–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lui JH, Hansen DV, Kriegstein AR (2011) Development and evolution of the human neocortex. Cell 146:18–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martínez Mateo M, Cabanis M, de Echeverría Cruz, Loebell N, Krach S (2012) Concerns about cultural neurosciences: a critical analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:152–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masuda T, Nisbett RE (2001) Attending holistically versus analytically: comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. J Pers Soc Psychol 81:922–934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathur VA, Harada T, Lipke T, Chiao JY (2010) Neural basis of extraordinary empathy and altruistic motivation. NeuroImage 51:1468–1475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCall C, Singer T (2012) The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social cognition, motivation and behavior. Nat Neurosci 15:681–688

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesquita B, Feldman Barrett L, Smith ER (2010) The mind in context. Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Montague RP, Berns GS, Cohen JD, McClure SM, Pagnoni G, Dhamala M, Wiest MC et al (2002) Hyperscanning: simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. NeuroImage 16:1159–1164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Northcutt GR (2002) Understanding vertebrate brain evolution. Integr Comp Biol 42:743–756

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA (2011) Super cooperators: altruism, evolution, and why we need each other to succeed. Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA (2012) Evolving cooperation. J Theor Biol 299:1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee JF, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom E, Walker J (2003) Trust and reciprocity: interdisciplinary lessons from experimental research. Russell Sage Foundation, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne C, Bachevalier J (2009) Neuroanatomy of the developing social brain. In: De Haan M, Gunnar M (eds) Handbook of developmental social neuroscience. The Guilford Press, New York, pp 38–62

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfaff DW (2007) The neuroscience of fair play: why we (usually) follow the golden rule. Dana Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps EA (2006) Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annu Rev Psychol 57:27–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps EA, LeDoux JE (2005) Contribution of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48:175–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pigliucci M, Muller GB (2010) Evolution—the extended synthesis. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rakic P (2009) Evolution of the neocortex: a perspective from developmental biology. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:724–735

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran VS (2011) The tell-tale brain: a neuroscientist’s quest for what makes us human. W. W. Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Raz A (2012) From neuroimaging to tea leaves in the bottom of a cup. In: Choudhury S, Slaby J (eds) Critical neuroscience: a handbook of the social and cultural context of neuroscience. Blackwell Publishing, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Ou S-R, Artega IA, White BAB (2011) School-based early childhood education and age-28 well being: effects by timing, dosage, and subgroups. Science 333:360–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey A, Dorris M (2009) Games in humans and non-human primates: scanners to single units. In: Glimcher PW, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Poldrack RA (eds) Neuroeconomics: decision making and the brain. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Schilhab T, Stjernfelt F, Deacon TW (2012) The symbolic species evolved. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shahaeian A, Peterson CC, Slaughter V, Wellman HM (2011) Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Dev Psychol 47:1239–1247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw CA, McEachern JC (2001) Toward a theory of neuroplasticity. Psychology Press, Philadelphia

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer T, Fehr E (2005) The neuroeconomics of mind reading and empathy: institute for empirical research in economics. University of Zurich, Zurich

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty JP, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2006) Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439:466–469

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skuse DH, Gallagher L (2011) Genetic influences on social cognition. Pediatr Res 69:85R–91R

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Star SL (1989) Regions of the mind: brain research and the quest for scientific certainty. Stanford University Press, Palo Alto

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephanova DI, Kolev BD (2013) Computational neuroscience: simulated demyelinating neuropathies and neuronopathies. CRC Press, Boca Raton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a Hostile World. Blackwell Publishing, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Striedter GF (2005) Principles of brain evolution. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Sudmant PH, Kitzman JO, Antonacci F, Alkan C, Malig M, Tsalenko A, Sampas N, Bruhn L, Shendure J, Eichler EE (2010) Diversity of human copy number variation and multicopy genes. Science 330:641–646

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suomi SJ (2009) How gene-environment interactions shape biobehavioural development: lessons from studies with rhesus monkeys. In: Tremblay RE, van Aken AG, Koops W (eds) Development and prevention of behaviour problems: from genes to social policy. Psychology Press, New York, pp 7–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Telzer EH, Humphreys K, Shapiro M, Tottenham N (2013) Amygdala sensitivity to race is not present in childhood but emerges over adolescence. J Cogn Neurosci 25:234–244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toga AW, Thompson PM, Sowell ER (2006) Mapping brain maturation. Trends Neurosci 29:148–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Uhlhaas PJ, Singer WJ (2011) Developmental changes in neuronal oscillations and synchrony: evidence for a lLate critical period. In: Human neuroplasticity and education, pontifical academy of sciences, Scripta Varia, vol 117, pp 218–260

  • Uludag K, Ugurbil K, Berliner L (2015) fMRI: from nuclear spins to brain functions (biological magnetic resonance). Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wedeen VJ, Rosene DL, Wang R, Dai G, Mortazavi F, Hagmann P, Kaas JH, Tseng W-IY (2012) The geometric structure of the brain fiber pathways. Science 335:1628

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise RA (2004) Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt W (1904) Volkerpsychologie: eine untersuchung der entwicklungsgesetze von sprache, mythos und sitte. Wilhelm Engelmann, Leipzig

    Google Scholar 

  • Zak PJ (2008) Moral markets: the critical role of values in the economy. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

First and foremost we would like to recognize intellectual debt that two of us (Larson and Sarto-Jackson) owe to our co-author Werner Callebaut. Sadly, we lost our dear friend in 2014. Werner’s keen intellect, analytical mind and philosopher’s perspective is greatly missed. He made any discussion about biology, evolution, epigenetics and human behavior more interesting and profound. But, it is his sense of humor, appreciation for fairness, and shared love of jazz that we also greatly miss.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Isabella Sarto-Jackson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sarto-Jackson, I., Larson, D.O. & Callebaut, W. Culture, neurobiology, and human behavior: new perspectives in anthropology. Biol Philos 32, 729–748 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-017-9574-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-017-9574-2

Keywords

Navigation