Skip to main content

Eye-Tracking und reaktionszeitbasierte Verfahren zur Messung impliziter Kommunikationswirkungen

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Book cover Handbuch Controlling der Kommunikation

Part of the book series: Springer NachschlageWissen ((SRW))

  • 710 Accesses

Zusammenfassung

In der Praxis geschieht die Erfassung von Kommunikationswirkungen überwiegend in Form von Befragungen, die jedoch tatsächlich nur einen Teil der Werbewirkung abbilden. Der Beitrag befasst sich vor diesem Hintergrund mit Messmethoden zur Erfassung impliziter Kommunikationswirkungen, die KonsumentInnen nicht bewusst wahrnehmen. Die Autoren beschreiben Methoden, die implizite Kommunikationswirkungen auf den Ebenen der Wahrnehmung und Abspeicherung von Information sowie des tatsächlichen Verhaltens sichtbar machen. Die Bedeutung von Blickbewegungsmessungen und reaktionszeitbasierten Verfahren zur Optimierung von Kommunikationsstrategien wird vor dem Hintergrund aktueller Literatur und anhand praxisrelevanter Beispiele aufgezeigt und diskutiert.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Literatur

  • Anderson, J. R., und G. H. Bower. 1973. Human associative memory. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bamford, S., und R. Ward. 2008. Predispositions to approach and avoid are contextually sensitive and goal dependent. Emotion 8(2): 174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, S., U. Wagner, und C. Schwand. 2012. Assessing advertising effectiveness: The potential of goal-directed behavior. Psychology & Marketing 29(6): 411–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bojko, A. 2013. Eye tracking the user experience: A practical guide to research. New York: Rosenfeld Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brendl, C. M., A. B. Markman, und C. Messner. 2005. Indirectly measuring evaluations of several attitude objects in relation to a neutral reference point. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41(4): 346–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, N. D., und J. K. Tsotsos. 2007. An information theoretic model of saliency and visual search. Attention in Cognitive Systems. Theories and Systems from an Interdisciplinary Viewpoint 4840: 171–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunel, F. F., B. C. Tietje, und A. G. Greenwald. 2004. Is the implicit association test a valid and valuable measure of implicit consumer social cognition? Journal of Consumer Psychology 14(4):385–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Büttner, O., A. Florack, und M. Scarabis. 2014. Werbekommunikation. In Psychologie der Kommunikation für Sozial- und Wirtschaftswissenschaftler, Hrsg. M. Blanz, A. Florack und U. Piontkowski, 194–203. Stuttgart: Vahlen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M., und J. A. Bargh. 1999. Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25: 215–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane, H. D. 1994. The Purkinje image eyetracker, image stabilization, and related forms of stimulus manipulation. In Visual science and engineering: models and applications, Hrsg. D. H. Kelly, 13–89. New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., K. J. Preacher, und M. R. Banaji. 2001. Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science 12(2): 163–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., S. Thomas, und F. Baeyens. 2001. Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin 127(6): 853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., S. Teige-Mocigemba, A. Spruyt, und A. Moors. 2009. Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin 135(3): 347–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delabarre, E. B. 1898. A method of recording eye-movements. The American Journal of Psychology 9(4): 572–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimofte, C. V. 2010. Implicit measures of consumer cognition: A review. Psychology & Marketing 27(10): 921–937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimofte, C. V., und R. F. Yalch. 2007. Consumer response to polysemous brand slogans. Journal of Consumer Research 33(4): 515–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, R., und T. S. Cline. 1901. The angle velocity of eye movements. Psychological Review 8(2): 145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duchowski, A. T. 2002. A breadth-first survey of eye-tracking applications. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 34(4): 455–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duchowski, A. T. 2007. Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice, 2. Aufl. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duchowski, A. T., N. Cournia, und H. Murphy. 2004. Gaze-contingent displays: A review. CyberPsychology & Behavior 7(6): 621–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fang, X., S. Singh, und R. Ahluwalia. 2007. An examination of different explanations for the mere exposure effect. Journal of Consumer Research 34(1): 97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. 1986. How do attitudes guide behavior? In The Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, Hrsg. R. M. Sorrentino und E. T. Higgins, 204–243. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro, R., J. R. Bettman, und T. L. Chartrand. 2009. The power of strangers: The effect of incidental consumer brand encounters on brand choice. Journal of Consumer Research 35(5): 729–741.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Florack, A., und M. Scarabis. 2004. Warum ein Werbespot nicht nur gefallen sollte – Multimethodale Erfassung von Werbewirkungen. Wirtschaftspsychologie Aktuell 11:39–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forehand, M. R., und A. Perkins. 2005. Implicit assimilation and explicit contrast: A set/reset model of response to celebrity voice-overs. Journal of Consumer Research 32(3): 435–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. J., M. X. Cohen, und A. G. Sanfey. 2009. Multiple systems in decision making: A neurocomputational perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18(2): 73–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. B., und N. Ambady. 2010. MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method. Behavior Research Methods 42(1): 226–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friese, M., W. Hofmann, und M. Wänke. 2008. When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology 47(3): 397–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genschow, O., A. Florack, V. S. Chib, S. Shimojo, M. Scarabis, und M. Wänke. 2013. Reaching for the (product) stars: Measuring recognition and approach speed to get insights into consumer choice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 35(3): 298–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, B. 2008. Can evaluative conditioning change attitudes toward mature brands? New evidence from the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Consumer Research 35(1): 178–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., und M. R. Banaji. 1995. Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review 102:4–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., D. E. McGhee, und J. L. K. Schwartz. 1998. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 1464–1480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., T. A. Poehlman, E. L. Uhlmann, und M. R. Banaji. 2009. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97(1): 17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M. 2014. Eye-tracking technology aims to take your unconscious pizza order. In Scientific American. http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/eye-tracking-technology-aims-to-take-your-unconscious-pizza-order/. Zugegriffen am 29.12.2014.

  • Hildenbrand, A., und R. Kühl. 2014. Ritter Sport und Stiftung Warentest: Informationsdefizite überwinden. Wirtschaftsdienst 94(3): 217–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmqvist, K., M. Nyström, R. Andersson, R. Dewhurst, H. Jarodzka, und J. Van de Weijer. 2011. Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures, 1. Aufl. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huey, E. B. 1898. Preliminary experiments in the physiology and psychology of reading. The American Journal of Psychology 9(4): 575–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Itti, L., und C. Koch. 2000. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research 40(10): 1489–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, R. J., und K. S. Karn. 2003. Eye tracking in human-computer interaction and usability research: Ready to deliver the promises. Mind 2(3): 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., und P. A. Carpenter. 1976. Eye fixations and cognitive processes. Cognitive Psychology 8(4): 441–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krajbich, I., C. Armel, und A. Rangel. 2010. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nature Neuroscience 13(10): 1292–1298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krieglmeyer, R., und R. Deutsch. 2010. Comparing measures of approach–avoidance behaviour: The manikin task vs. two versions of the joystick task. Cognition and Emotion 24(5): 810–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishna, A. 2012. An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology 22:332–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishnan, H. S. 1996. Characteristics of memory associations: A consumer-based brand equity perspective. International Journal of Research in Marketing 13(4): 389–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Meur, O., und T. Baccino. 2013. Methods for comparing scanpaths and saliency maps: Strengths and weaknesses. Behavior Research Methods 45(1): 251–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maison, D., A. G. Greenwald, und R. H. Bruin. 2004. Predictive validity of the Implicit Association Test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology 14(4): 405–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meissner, F., und K. Rothermund. 2013. Estimating the contributions of associations and recoding in the Implicit Association Test: The ReAL model for the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 104(1): 45–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H., D. E. Keefe, und K. L. Ross. 1989. Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: Roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies and retro-spective semantic matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15:1003–1019.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., C. B. Hawkins, und R. S. Frazier. 2011. Implicit social cognition: From measures to mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(4): 152–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orquin, J. L., und S. Mueller Loose. 2013. Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychologica 144(1): 190–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oskamp, S., und P. W. Schultz. 2005. Attitudes and opinions, 3. Aufl. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pieters, R., und L. Warlop. 1999. Visual attention during brand choice: The impact of time pressure and task motivation. International Journal of Research in Marketing 16(1): 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pieters, R., und M. Wedel. 2008. Informativeness of eye movements for visual marketing: six cornerstones. In Visual marketing – From attention to action, Hrsg. M. Wedel und R. Pieters. 1. Aufl., New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., und M. S. Castelhano. 2008. Eye movements during reading, scene perception, visual search, and while looking at print advertisement. In Visual marketing – From attention to action, Hrsg. M. Wedel und R. Pieters, 1. Aufl., 9–42. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridder, W. H., und A. Tomlinson. 1997. A comparison of saccadic and blink suppression in normal observers. Vision Research 37(22): 3171–3179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarabis, M., und A. Florack. 2003. Was denkt der Konsument wirklich? Reaktionszeitbasierte Verfahren als Instrument der Markenanalyse. Planung & Analyse 6: 30–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarabis, M., und A. Florack. 2007. Neue Einsichten durch neue Methoden: Reaktionszeitbasierte Verfahren in der Marken und Werbeforschung. In Psychologie der Markenführung, Hrsg. A. Florack, M. Scarabis und E. Primosch, 463–483. München: Vahlen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimojo, S., C. Simion, E. Shimojo, und C. Scheier. 2003. Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience 6(12): 1317–1322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slabbinck, H., J. De Houwer, und P. Van Kenhove. 2011. A pictorial attitude IAT as a measure of implicit motives. European Journal of Personality 25(1): 76–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sriram, N., und A. G. Greenwald. 2009. The brief implicit association test. Experimental Psychology 56(4): 283–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., und R. Deutsch. 2004. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review 8: 220–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dantzig, S., D. Pecher, und R. A. Zwaan. 2008. Approach and avoidance as action effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61(9): 1298–1306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wedel, M. 2013. Attention research in marketing: A review of eye tracking studies. Robert H. Smith School Research Paper No. RHS, 2460289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wedel, M., und R. Pieters. 2000. Eye fixations on advertisements and memory for brands: A model and findings. Marketing Science 19(4): 297–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wedel, M., und R. Pieters. 2006. Eye tracking for visual marketing. Foundations and Trends in Marketing 1(4): 231–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wedel, M., und R. Pieters. 2008. A review of eye-tracking research in marketing. Review of Marketing Research 4: 123–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittenbrink, B. 2007. Measuring attitudes through priming. In Implicit measures of attitudes, Hrsg. B. Wittenbrink und N. Schwarz, 17–58. New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittenbrink, B., C. M. Judd, und B. Park. 1997. Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72(2): 262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johanna Palcu .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Gabler Verlag | Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH, Wiesbaden

About this entry

Cite this entry

Palcu, J., Florack, A. (2015). Eye-Tracking und reaktionszeitbasierte Verfahren zur Messung impliziter Kommunikationswirkungen. In: Esch, FR., Langner, T., Bruhn, M. (eds) Handbuch Controlling der Kommunikation. Springer NachschlageWissen. Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-05260-7_11-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-05260-7_11-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-658-05260-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Referenz Wirtschaftswissenschaften

Publish with us

Policies and ethics