Skip to main content

Paired-Associate Learning

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology

Synonyms

Associative memory; Episodic memory; Relational learning

Definition

Paired-associate learning (PAL) is an episodic memory paradigm in which pairs of items (e.g., “absence-hollow”) are presented during one or more learning trials. At test, the first item of the pair is presented as a cue in order to elicit a response of the second item. Memory for the test items by themselves is insufficient to support accurate PAL memory performance; rather, accurate PAL is based on whether the paired items have been associated, or bound together, in memory. PAL was first described by Mary Whiton Calkins in 1894, a time during which all learning was viewed as associations between stimuli and responses, and reflects classic “stimulus-response” assumptions that dominated experimental psychology from the early-to mid-to late twentieth century. Today, there is much interest in PAL, as psychologists view this type of memory paradigm to be representative of the kind of learning undertaken in...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References and Readings

  • Bishop, D. V., & Hsu, H. J. (2015). The declarative system in children with specific language impairment: A comparison of meaningful and meaningless auditory-visual paired associate learning. BioMed Central Psychology, 3(1), 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. A., Sagar, H. J., Jordan, N., Harvey, N. S., & Sullivan, E. V. (1991). Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson’s disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain, 114, 2095–2122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delotterie, D., Mathis, C., Cassel, J. C., Dorner-Ciossek, C., & Marti, A. (2014). Optimization of touchscreen-based behavioral paradigms in mice: Implications for building a battery of tasks taxing learning and memory functions. PloS One, 9(6), e100817.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, K. S., Saling, M. M., Conway, E. L., Semple, J. M., & Louis, W. J. (2002). Paired associate performance in the early detection of DAT. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 58–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, I., & Adams, K. M. (2009). Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric and neuromedical disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and associative learning. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Goetz, K., Gooch, D., Adams, J., & Snowling, M. J. (2007). Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(2), 150–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ivison, D. (1977). The Wechsler Memory Scale: Preliminary findings toward an Australian standardization. Australian Psychologist, 12, 303–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivnik, R. J. (1991). Memory testing. In T. Yanagihara & R. C. Petersen (Eds.), Memory disorders: Research and clinical practice. New York: Marcel Dekker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarema, G., & Libben, G. (2015). Phonological and phonetic considerations of lexical processing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kee, D. W., & Rohwer, W. D. (1973). Noun-pair learning in four ethnic groups: Conditions of presentation and response. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 226–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, W. J., Horwitz, B., Taylor, J. G., Schmidt, D., Mottaghy, F. M., Herzog, H., Halsband, U., & Müller-Gärtner, H. (1999). Network analysis in episodic encoding and retrieval of word-pair associates: A PET study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11(9), 3293–3301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurtz, T., Mogle, J., Sliwinski, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2013). Individual differences in task-specific paired associates learning in older adults: The role of processing speed and working memory. Experimental Aging Research, 39(5), 493–514.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M., Howieson, D., & Loring, D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Litt, R. A., de Jong, P. F., van Bergen, E., & Nation, K. (2013). Dissociating crossmodal and verbal demands in paired associate learning (PAL): What drives the PAL–reading relationship? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 137–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitrushina, M. N., Boone, K. B., & D’Elia, L. F. (1999). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment (1st ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottaghy, F. M., Shah, N. J., Krause, B. J., Schmidt, D., Halsband, U., Jäncke, L., & Müller-Gärtner, H. W. (1999). Neuronal correlates of encoding and retrieval in episodic memory during a paired-word association learning task: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Experimental Brain Research, 128(3), 332–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1170–1187.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, H., Coen, R., Kidd, N., Warsi, M., Chin, A. V., & Lawlor, B. A. (2004). Early detection of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) using the CANTAB paired associates learning test. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19, 1207–1208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Provyn, J., Sliwinski, M., & Howard, M. (2007). Effects of age on contextually mediated associations in paired associate learning. Psychology and Aging, 22, 846–857.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Puff, C. R. (1982). Handbook of research methods in human memory and cognition. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., McInnes, L., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1994). Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB): A factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia, 5, 266–281.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, M., Allendorfer, J. B., Lindsell, C. J., Vannest, J., & Szaflarski, J. P. (2012). The effects of linguistic relationships among paired associates on verbal self-generation and recognition memory. Brain and Behavior, 2(6), 789–795.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Characterizing amnesic patients for neurobehavioral study. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 866–877.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253, 1380–1386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Talpos, J., Aerts, N., Waddell, J., & Steckler, T. (2015). MK-801 and amphetamine result in dissociable profiles of cognitive impairment in a rodent paired associates learning task with relevance for schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology, 232, 21–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2005). The Oxford handbook of memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vannest, J., Maloney, T., Kay, B., Siegel, M., Allendorfer, J. B., Banks, C., & Szaflarski, J. P. (2015). Age related-changes in the neural basis of self-generation in verbal paired associate learning. NeuroImage: Clinical, 7, 537–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warmington, M., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phoneme awareness, visual-verbal paired-associate learning, and rapid automatized naming as predictors of individual differences in reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(1), 45–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for clinical use. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 19, 87–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weniger, G., Boucsein, K., & Irle, E. (2004). Impaired associative memory in temporal lobe epilepsy subjects after lesions of hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, and amygdala. Hippocampus, 14, 785–796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wild, K., Howieson, D., Webbe, F., Seelye, A., & Kaye, J. (2008). The status of computerized cognitive testing in aging: A systematic review. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association, 4(6), 428–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windfuhr, K. L., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(2), 160–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kerri A. Scorpio .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this entry

Cite this entry

Scorpio, K.A., Islam, R., Kim, S.M., Bind, R., Borod, J.C., Bender, H.A. (2017). Paired-Associate Learning. In: Kreutzer, J., DeLuca, J., Caplan, B. (eds) Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_1137-2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_1137-2

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-56782-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-56782-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics