Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Affiliative Bond

  • Pawel FedurekEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_717-1

Synonyms

Definition

Long-term relationships of individuals, characterized by a high degree of proximity and high rates of friendly interactions, are termed affiliative or social “bonds.” The process by which such relationships are established and maintained is termed “social bonding.”

Adaptive Significance

Evolutionarily, the oldest bond is probably that between mother and offspring, and emerged when postnatal maternal care became crucial for offspring survival. Mother-infant bonds are particularly important in species where offspring’s dependence on the mother is a prolonged and energetically costly process, as in some birds and especially in mammals. In the latter, bonds between the mother and infant emerge during the earliest stages of ontogenetic development to facilitate nursing, and often, as in humans, extend well beyond the lactation period to ensure offspring’s survival into reproductive age. In animals...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Crockford, C., Deschner, T., Ziegler, T. E., & Wittig, R. M. (2014). Endogenous peripheral oxytocin measures can give insight into the dynamics of social relationships: A review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2010). Bondedness and sociality. Behaviour, 147(7), 775–803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Fedurek, P., Machanda, Z. P., Schel, A. M., & Slocombe, K. E. (2013). Pant hoot chorusing and social bonds in male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 86(1), 189–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. McFarland, R., Murphy, D., Lusseau, D., Henzi, S. P., Parker, J. L., Pollet, T. V., & Barrett, L. (2017). The ‘strength of weak ties’ among female baboons: Fitness-related benefits of social bonds. Animal Behaviour, 126, 101–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Muller, M. N., & Mitani, J. C. (2005). Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. In P. J. B. Slater, J. Rosenblatt, C. Snowdon, T. Roper, & M. Naguib (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (pp. 275–331). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  6. Samuni, L., Preis, A., Mundry, R., Deschner, T., Crockford, C., & Wittig, R. M. (2017). Oxytocin reactivity during intergroup conflict in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(2), 268–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 153–177.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Silk, J. B., Beehner, J. C., Bergman, T. J., Crockford, C., Engh, A. L., Moscovice, L. R., … Cheney, D. L. (2010). Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current Biology, 20(15), 1359–1361.Google Scholar
  9. Smith, W. J. (1994). Animal duets: Forcing a mate to be attentive. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 166(2), 221–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Lehmann, J., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2008). Focused grooming networks and stress alleviation in wild female baboons. Hormones and Behavior, 54(1), 170–177.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PrimatologyMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany

Section editors and affiliations

  • Caroline Leuchtenberger
    • 1
  1. 1.Federal Institute of Education, Science and TechnologySanta MariaBrazil