Absolute Pitch
- 261 Downloads
Synonyms
Definition
In humans, the ability to name or vocally produce any musical note without using a reference note. More broadly, the ability to accurately remember auditory pitch not just in terms of the relationships among pitches.
Introduction
If you ask a musician for an example of a musical talent or gift, one of the most common answers would be absolute pitch (AP), and for good reason. Typically defined as the ability to name or produce musical notes without the need for a starting reference, AP is thought to be exceedingly rare, with an estimated incidence of occurrence of around 1 in 10,000 people (Bachem 1955). While this number may vary substantially across cultures (Miyazaki et al. 2012), it is clear that AP is disproportionately present in top music conservatories across the world (cf. Deutsch et al. 2006), and moreover, a number of well-known composers and musicians, from Mozart to Mariah Carey, have reportedly possessed AP. Yet, despite over a century of...
References
- Athos, E. A., Levinson, B., Kistler, A., Zemansky, J., Bostrom, A., Freimer, N., & Gitschier, J. (2007). Dichotomy and perceptual distortions in absolute pitch ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(37), 14795–14800. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703868104.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Bachem, A. (1937, May). Various types of absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 9, 146–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bachem, A. (1940). The genesis of absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 11, 434–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bachem, A. (1955). Absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(6), 1180–1185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Baharloo, S., Service, S. K., Risch, N., Gitschier, J., & Freimer, N. B. (2000). Familial aggregation of absolute pitch. American Journal of Human Genetics, 67(3), 755–758. https://doi.org/10.1086/303057.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Bahr, N., Christensen, C. A., & Bahr, M. (2005). Diversity of accuracy profiles for absolute pitch recognition. Psychology of Music, 33(1), 58–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735605048014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bermudez, P., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). A distribution of absolute pitch ability as revealed by computerized testing. Music Perception, 27(2), 89–101. https://doi.org/10.1525/rep.2008.104.1.92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brady, P. T. (1970). Fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48(4), 883–887. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1912227.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., & Dolson, M. (2004). Absolute pitch, speech, and tone language: Some experiments and a proposed framework. Music Perception, 21(3), 339–356. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., & Xu, H. (2006). Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences, and evidence for a speech-related critical period. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(2), 719–722. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2151799.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Deutsch, D., Le, J., Shen, J., & Li, X. (2011). Large-scale direct-test study reveals unexpected characteristics of absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(4), 2398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dohn, A., Garza-Villarreal, E. A., Riisgaard Ribe, L., Wallentin, M., & Vuust, P. (2014). Musical activity tunes up absolute pitch ability. Music Perception, 31(4), 359–371. https://doi.org/10.1525/rep.2008.104.1.92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Friedrich, A., Zentall, T., & Weisman, R. (2007). Absolute pitch: Frequency-range discriminations in pigeons (Columba livia) – Comparisons with zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(1), 95–105. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.121.1.95.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Gervain, J., Vines, B. W., Chen, L. M., Seo, R. J., Hensch, T. K., Werker, J. F., & Young, A. H. (2013). Valproate reopens critical-period learning of absolute pitch. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7(102), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hamilton, R. H., Pascual-Leone, A., & Schlaug, G. (2004). Absolute pitch in blind musicians. Neuroreport, 15(5), 803–806. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200404090-00012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hedger, S. C., Heald, S. L. M., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2013). Absolute pitch may not be so absolute. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1496–1502. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612473310.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Joseph, J. (2002). Twin studies in psychiatry and psychology: Science or pseudoscience? Psychiatric Quarterly, 73(1), 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012896802713.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lee, C.-Y., & Lee, Y.-F. (2011). Perception of musical and lexical tones by Taiwanese-speaking musicians. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(1), 526–535. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4754918.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Levitin, D. J., & Rogers, S. E. (2005). Absolute pitch: Perception, coding, and controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 26–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lewandowsky, S., Yang, L.-X., Newell, B. R., & Kalish, M. L. (2012). Working memory does not dissociate between different perceptual categorization tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 881–904. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027298.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lockhead, G., & Byrd, R. (1981). Practically perfect pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 70(2), 387–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Miyazaki, K. (1990). The speed of musical pitch identification possessors by absolute-pitch possessors. Music Perception, 8(2), 177–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Miyazaki, K., Makomaska, S., & Rakowski, A. (2012). Prevalence of absolute pitch: A comparison between Japanese and Polish music students. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(5), 3484–3493. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4756956.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rush, M. (1989). An experimental investigation of the effectiveness of training on absolute pitch in adult musicians. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.Google Scholar
- Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (2003). Good pitch memory is widespread. Psychological Science, 14(3), 262–266. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.03432.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Takeuchi, A. H., & Hulse, S. H. (1993). Absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 345–361. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.345.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Theusch, E., & Gitschier, J. (2011). Absolute pitch twin study and segregation analysis. Twin Research and Human Genetics : The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies, 14(2), 173–178. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.14.2.173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Theusch, E., Basu, A., & Gitschier, J. (2009). Genome-wide study of families with absolute pitch reveals linkage to 8q24.21 and locus heterogeneity. American Journal of Human Genetics, 85(1), 112–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.06.010.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, S. L. M., Koch, R., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2015a). Auditory working memory predicts individual differences in absolute pitch learning. Cognition, 140, 95–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, S. L. M., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2015b). The effects of acoustic variability on absolute pitch categorization : Evidence of contextual tuning. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(1), 436–446. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4922952.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, S. L. M., Huang, A., Rutstein, B., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2016a). Telling in-tune from out-of-tune: Widespread evidence for implicit absolute intonation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(2), 481–488. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1099-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, S. L. M., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2016b). What the [bleep]? Enhanced absolute pitch memory for a 1000 Hz sine tone. Cognition, 154, 139–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.06.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, S. L. M., Uddin, S., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2018). A note by any other name: Intonation context rapidly changes absolute note judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000536.
- Vanzella, P., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). Absolute pitch: Effects of timbre on note-naming ability. PLoS One, 5(11). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Vitouch, O. (2003). Absolutist models of absolute pitch are absolutely misleading. Music Perception, 21(1), 111–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ward, W. D. (1999). Absolute pitch. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 265–298). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ward, W. D., & Burns, E. M. (1982). Absolute pitch. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (1st ed., pp. 431–451). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weisman, R. G., Balkwill, L. L., Hoeschele, M., Moscicki, M. K., Bloomfield, L. L., & Sturdy, C. B. (2010). Absolute pitch in boreal chickadees and humans: Exceptions that test a phylogenetic rule. Learning and Motivation, 41(3), 156–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2010.04.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weisman, R. G., Balkwill, L.-L., Hoeschele, M., Moscicki, M. K., & Sturdy, C. B. (2012). Identifying absolute pitch possessors without using a note-naming task. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 22(1), 46–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wilson, S. J., Lusher, D., Martin, C. L., Rayner, G., & McLachlan, N. (2012). Intersecting factors lead to absolute pitch acquisition that is maintained in a “fixed do” environment. Music Perception, 29(3), 285–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Yang, E.-J., Lin, E. W., & Hensch, T. K. (2012). Critical period for acoustic preference in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Supplement_2), 17213–17220. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200705109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zatorre, R. J. (2003). Absolute pitch: A model for understanding the influence of genes and development on neural and cognitive function. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 692–695. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1085.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar