Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Acquisition

  • Martha EscobarEmail author
  • Francisco Arcediano
  • Chukwuebuka Unobagha
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1058-1

Definition

Gradual development of an association between events that are encountered together.

Introduction

Events that have biological relevance to the organism can elicit reflexive responses (e.g., food can elicit salivation). Through a similar process, organisms may produce responses in order to bring forth or prevent the occurrence of a consequence (e.g., following a command to obtain a food reward). In either situation, a link or association between the two events develops, and responses consistent with this association are observed. These responses have the important function of preparing the organism for an impending biologically relevant event or better suiting the organism to maximize reward and minimize punishment in its environment. The process by which paired events become associated is known as acquisition.

What Relationships Are Acquired?

Learning about environmental events can be broadly classified as occurring through classical conditioning or instrumental conditioning....

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brogden, W. J. (1939). Sensory pre-conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(4), 323–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Miller, R. R., & Escobar, M. (2001). Contrasting acquisition-focused and performance-focused models of acquired behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(4), 141–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, Trans.). London: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  5. Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66(1), 1–5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  7. Rogan, M. T., Stäubli, U. V., & LeDoux, J. E. (1997). Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature, 390, 604–607.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Savastano, H. I., Cole, R. P., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1999). Reconsidering conditioned inhibition. Learning and Motivation, 30(1), 101–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  10. Wagner, A. R., & Brandon, S. E. (2001). A componential theory of Pavlovian conditioning. In R. R. Mowrer & S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp. 23–64). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Martha Escobar
    • 1
    Email author
  • Francisco Arcediano
    • 1
  • Chukwuebuka Unobagha
    • 1
  1. 1.Oakland UniversityRochesterUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Mark A. Krause
    • 1
  1. 1.Southern Oregon UniversityAshlandUSA