Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Hormone Effects on Human Fetal Development

  • Catherine DeSotoEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3630-1

Prenatal Programming, Cortisol

The evidence that phenotype is set partially based on prenatal hormonal exposure is overall strong. Such effects make sense within the evolutionary psychology framework.

Hormone levels that a developing fetus experiences can result in long-lasting phenotypic differences in the individual. Because the maternal blood supply is the route by which a fetus will be exposed, mother’s levels – and her experiences – can affect development.

High levels of stress during pregnancy result in higher levels of the hormone cortisol as does exogenous ingestion of certain medicines (e.g., steroids) or certain foods (i.e., licorice). In any case, there is evidence that exposure of the fetus during prenatal development will alter early programming, causing differences in metabolism and other physiological parameters. The effects are long term or permanent. Combined with experimental methods employing animal models (see Weinstock 2008), there is converging evidence...

Keywords

Evolutionary Psychology Hormonal Exposure Excess Cortisol Hormone Cortisol Psychology Framework 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bergman, K., Sarkar, P., Glover, V., & O’Connor, T. G. (2010). Maternal prenatal cortisol and infant cognitive development: Moderation by infant mother attachment. Biological Psychology, 67(11), 1026–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Buss, C., Poggi-Davis, E., Shahbaba, B., Pruessner, J. C., Head, J., & Sandman, C. A. (2012). Maternal cortisol over the course of pregnancy and subsequent child amygdala and hippocampus volumes and affective problems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1312–1319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Li, Y., He, Y., Qi, L., Jaddoe, V. W., Feskens, E. J. M., Yang, X., Ma, G., & Hu, F. B. (2010). Exposure to the Chinese famine in early life and the risk of hyperglycemia and type 2 diabetes in adulthood. Diabetes, 59, 2400–2406.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. O’Connor, T. G., Bengman, K., Sorkar, P., & Glover, V. (2013). Prenatal cortisol exposure predicts infant cortisol response to acute stress. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(2), 145–155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Reynolds, R. M. (2010). Corticosteroid mediated programming and the pathogenesis of obesity and diabetes. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 122, 3–9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Reynolds, R. M. (2013). Glucocorticoid excess and the developing origins of disease: Two decades of testing the hypothesis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 1–11.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Weinstock, M. (2008). The long-term behavioral consequences of prenatal stress. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(6), 1073–1086.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.PsychologyUniversity of Northern IowaCedar FallsUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Jennifer Byrd-Craven
    • 1
  1. 1.Oklahoma State UniversityStillwaterUSA