Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Infanticide in Nonhumans

  • Mateo Peñaherrera-AguirreEmail author
  • Aurelio José Figueredo
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3047-1

Synonyms

Definition

A set of aggressive behaviors destined toward the elimination of younglings, usually a dependent infant or hatchling. The attacker often obtains a fitness benefit by targeting the offspring of conspecifics. Due to the costs associated with losing a youngling, parents often employ an array of physiological and behavioral tactics to decrease the risk of infanticide.

Introduction

Although nowadays research on nonhuman infanticide has lost most of its controversial nature, three decades ago studies on this subject generated heated debate concerning the adaptive function of eliminating infants (Sommer 2000). Due to the costs associated with producing and rearing offspring, killing younglings was interpreted as a pathological behavior with no apparent adaptive benefits. This...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Digby, L. (2000). Infanticide by female mammals: Implications for the evolution of social systems. In C. van Schaik & C. Janson (Eds.), Infanticide by males and its implications (pp. 423–446). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Hrdy, S. B. (1979). Infanticide among animals: A review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(1), 13–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Mock, D. W. (2004). More than kin and less than kind: The evolution of family conflict. Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Mock, D. W., & Forbes, L. S. (1994). Life-history consequences of avian brood reduction. The Auk, 111, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Opie, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Dunbar, R. I., & Shultz, S. (2013). Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(33), 13328–13332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Palombit, R. A. (1999). Infanticide and the evolution of pair bonds in nonhuman primates. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews: Issues, News, and Reviews, 7(4), 117–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Palombit, R. A. (2012). Infanticide: Male strategies and female counterstrategies. In J. C. Mitani, J. Call, P. M. Kappeler, R. A. Palombit, R. A, J. B. Silk, & J. B (Eds.), The evolution of primate societies (pp. 432–468). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  8. Sommer, V. (2000). The holy wars about infanticide. Which side are you on? And why? In C. Van Schaik & C. Janson (Eds.), Infanticide by males and its implications (pp. 9–26). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection (Vol. 136, p. 179). Cambridge, MA: Biological Laboratories, Harvard University.Google Scholar
  10. Van Schaik, C. P. (2000). Vulnerability to infanticide by males: Patterns among mammals. In C. Van Schaik & C. Janson (Eds.), Infanticide by males and its implications (pp. 61–72). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mateo Peñaherrera-Aguirre
    • 1
    Email author
  • Aurelio José Figueredo
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of ArizonaTucsonUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Catherine Salmon
    • 1
  1. 1.University of RedlandsRedlandsUSA