Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Savanna Hypothesis, The

  • Gordon H. OriansEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2930-1

Synonyms

Definition

The Savanna Hypothesis states that we retain genetically based preferences for features of high-quality African savannas where our ancestors lived when their brains and bodies evolved into their modern forms.

Introduction

Selection of a place to live is a crucial step in the lives of most animals. Selection depends on the recognition of objects, sounds, and odors to which an animal, molded by natural selection, responds as if it understood their significance for its future survival and reproductive success. Evolutionary theory suggests that the ability of a landscape to evoke positive emotional states should be positively correlated with the expected survival and reproductive success of individuals of that species in it. In other words, good habitats should evoke strong positive responses; poor habitats should evoke weak or negative responses (Orians and Heerwagen 1992). Habitat selection has served as a conceptual basis for...

Keywords

African Savanna Tropical Savanna Landscape Preference Savanna Tree Herbaceous Understory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of landscape. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  2. Balling, J. D., & Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preferences for natural environments. Environment and Behavior, 14, 5–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bernáldez, F., Gallardo, D., & Abelló, R. P. (1987). Children’s landscape preferences: From rejection to attraction. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 7, 169–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Campbell, B. (1985). Human evolution (3rd ed.). New York: Aldine.Google Scholar
  5. Coss, R. G. (2003). The role of evolved perceptual biases in art and design. In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 69–130). Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Coss, R. G., & Goldthwaite, R. O. (1995). The persistence of old designs for perception. In N. S. Thompson (Ed.), Perspectives in ethology, volume 11: Behavioral design (pp. 83–148). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  7. Coss, R. G., & Moore, M. (2002). Precocious knowledge of trees as antipredator refuge in preschool children: An examination of aesthetics, attributive judgments, and relic sexual dinichism. Ecological Psychology, 14, 181–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Heerwagen, J. H., & Orians, G. H. (1993). Humans, habitats, and aesthetics. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (Eds.), The biophilia hypothesis (pp. 138–172). Washington, DC: Island Press.Google Scholar
  9. Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Marean, C. W., Milo, R. G., & Yates, R. (2001). An early bone tool industry from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa: Implications for the origins of modern human behaviour, symbolism and language. Journal of Human Evolution, 41, 631–678.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Herzog, T. R. (1985). A cognitive analysis of preference for waterscapes. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 225–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Komar, V., & Melamid, A. (1997). In J. A. Wypijewski (Ed.), Painting by the numbers: Komar and Melamid’s scientific guide to art. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
  12. Lohr, V. I., & Pearson-Mims, C. H. (2006). Responses to scenes with spreading, rounded, and conical tree forms. Environment and Behavior, 38, 667–668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lyons, E. (1983). Demographic correlates of landscape preference. Environment and Behavior, 15, 487–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Mellaart, J. (1968). Çatal Hüyük: A neolithic town in Anatolia. London: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
  15. Nabhan, G. P. (2004). Why some like it hot. Food, genes, and cultural diversity. Washington, DC: Island Press.Google Scholar
  16. Orians, G. H. (1980). Habitat selection. In J. S. Lockard (Ed.), The evolution of human social behavior (pp. 49–66). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  17. Orians, G. H. (2014). Snakes, sunrises and Shakespeare. How evolution shapes our loves and fears. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 555–579). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Repton, H. (1907). The art of landscape gardening. Boston/New York: Houghton Mifflin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ross, S. (1998). What gardens mean. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  21. Shanes, E. (1979). Turner’s picturesque views of England and Wales, 1825–1838. London: Chatto & Windus.Google Scholar
  22. Shuttleworth, S. (1980). The use of photographs as an environmental presentation medium in landscape studies. Journal of Environmental Management, 11, 61–76.Google Scholar
  23. Sommer, R. (1997). Further cross-national studies of tree-form preference. Ecological Psychology, 9, 153–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Sommer, R., & Summit, J. (1996). Cross-national rankings of tree shape. Ecological Psychology, 8, 327–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Tuan, Y.-F. (1974). Topophilia. A study of environmental perception, attitudes, and values. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  26. Ulrich, R. S. (1981). Natural versus urban spaces: Some psychophysiological effects. Environment and Behavior, 13, 523–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Wilson, M. E., Robertson, L. D., Daley, M., & Walton, S. A. (1995). Effects of visual cues on assessment of water quality. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 53–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Witzel, M. (2015). Water in mythology. Daedalus, 144, 18–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching fire. How cooking made us human. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  30. Zube, E. H., Simcox, D. E., & Law, C. S. (1987). Perceptual landscape simulations: History and prospect. Landscape Journal, 6, 62–80.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of WashingtonSeattleUSA