Skip to main content

Language Acquisition

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
  • 147 Accesses

Synonyms

First language acquisition; Language development; Primary language acquisition

Definition

Process of acquiring the sound system, structures, rules, and symbolic representations of a language and their meanings.

Introduction

How typically developing children universally acquire their primary language(s) with ease before any or without formal schooling is the foundation of research in language acquisition. There have been two general strands of research: nativist and nonnativist. The nativist approach proposes that an innate module gives humans a priori knowledge about the nature of language, specifically syntax, and results in the universal acquisition of language among all typically developing children (Chomsky 1957, 1975). The second strand assumes that there is no innate mechanism specifically dedicated to language, but innate social and cognitive abilities that have evolved for other purposes. There has been no clear winner as research from other disciplines examining...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

References

  • Akhtar, N., & Montague, L. (1999). Early lexical acquisition: The role of cross-situational learning. First Language, 19, 347–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8, 199–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72(2), 416–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, R., Schwade, J. R., & Goldstein, M. (2017). The social functions of babbling: Acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal responsiveness. Developmental Science, e12641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C., & Theakston, A. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 239–273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Astin, J. L. (1960). How to do things with words. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Attneave, F. (1953). Psychological probability as function of experienced frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46(2), 81–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, A., Dewar, B., Critchley, H., Dolan, R., Shallice, T., & Cipolotti, L. (2006). Social and emotional functions in three patients with medial frontal lobe damage including the anterior cingulate cortex. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11(4), 369–388.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19, 241–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, H., & Mollon, J. D. (Eds.). (1982). The senses. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 293–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, M. C. (1979). The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development. In M. Bullowas (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 63–77). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begus, K., Gliga, T., & Southgate, V. (2014). Infants learn what they want to learn: Responding to infant pointing leads to superior learning. PLoS One, 9(10), e108817.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bijeljac-Babic, R., Bertocini, J., & Mehler, J. (1991). How do four-day-old infants categorize multisyllabic utterances? Developmental Psychology, 29, 711–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blass, E., & Shah, A. (1994). Pain-reducing properties of sucrose in human newborns. Chemical Senses, 20(1), 29–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyland, J. (Ed.). (2001). Hypercorrect pronoun case in English? Cognitive processes that account for pronoun usage. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T., & Cramer, B. (1990). The earliest relationship: Parents, infants, and the Drama of early attachment. Reading: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickner, R. (1936). The intellectual functions of the frontal lobes: Study based upon observation of a man after a partial bilateral frontal loboctomy. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broesch, T., & Bryant, G. (2017). Father’s infants directed speech in a small-scale society. Child Development, 89, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brugger, A., Lariviere, L. A., Mumme, D. L., & Bushnell, E. (2007). Doing the right thing: Infants’ selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. Child Development, 78(3), 806–824.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, G., & Barrett, H. C. (2007). Recognizing intentions in infant-directed speech: Evidence for universals. Psychological Science, 18(8), 746–751.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J., & Hopper, P. (Eds.). (2001). Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, G., & Butterfield, E. C. (1975). Assessing infants’ auditory functioning. In B. Z. Friedlander (Ed.), Exceptional infant (Vol. 2, pp. 84–108). New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, C.-Y. (2006). The neurobiology of frequency effects. Los Angeles: UCLA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, B., & Fisher, C. (1998). Long-term auditory word priming in preschoolers: Implicit memory support for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 523–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584–1595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20(4), 477–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cristia, A. (2011). Fine-grained variation in caregivers’ predicts their infants’ category. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129, 3271–3280.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Attention and performance XXI: Processes of change in brain and cognitive development (pp. 249–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dabrowski, E. (2016). What exactly is Universal Grammar? and has anyone seen it? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 852.

    Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 28(3), 313–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, R. (1975). Auditory competence in early life: The roots of communicative behavior. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R., & Ellingson, R. (1972). Responses to electrical stimulation of the median nerve in the human newborn. Developmental Psychobiology, 6(3), 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erting, C., Prezioso, C., & O'Grady-Hynes, M. (1990). The interactional context of deaf mother-infant communication. In V. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 97–106). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Everett, D. (2012). Language: The cultural tool. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, D. (2009). Finding our tongues. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farran, L., Lee, C., Yoo, H., & Oller, D. K. (2017). Cross-cultural register differences in infant-directed speech: An initial study. PLoS One, 11(3), e0151518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 9602–9605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Massaccesi, S., Pividori, D., & Johnson, M. (2004). Gaze following in newborns. Infancy, 5(1), 39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. (1978). Talking to children: A search for universals. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers’ speech to infants. Child Development, 64(3), 637–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C., Hunt, C., Chambers, K., & Church, B. (2001). Abstraction and specificity in preschoolers’ representations of novel spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(4), 665–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Floor, P., & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversation? Infancy, 9(3), 327–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies, 6, 463–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, A., Farago, T., Galambos, A., & Topal, J. (2017). Differential effects of speech situations on mothers’ and fathers’ infant-directed and dog-directed speech: An scoustic analysis. Nature, 7, 13739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gliga, T., & Csibra, G. (2009). One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychological Science, 20(3), 347–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Wagner Alibali, M. (2013). Gesture‘s role in speaking learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, G., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2009). The philosophical baby: What children's minds tell us about truth, love, and the meaning of life. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hallan, N. (2001). Paths to prepositions? A corpus-based study of the acquisition of a lexico-grammatical category. In J. Bybee & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structures (pp. 91–122). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. (1975). Learning how to mean. In E. Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development (pp. 239–265). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Chromiak, W. (1977). The processing of frequency information: An automatic mechanism? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 173–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton, T., Wyland, C. L., Macrae, C. N., Demos, K. E., Denny, B. T., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 18–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2016). Born pupils? Natural pedagogy and cultural pedagogy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 280–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge, MA/London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horgan, J. (2016). Is Chomsky’s theory of language wrong? Pinker weighs in on debate. In Scientific American blog network. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornak, J., Rolls, E. T., & Wade, D. (1996). Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioral changes following ventral lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 34(4), 247–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P. (2004). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 182–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. (1978). Emotions and motivations: An evolutionary-developmental perspective. In H. Howe (Ed.), Nebraska symposium of motivation (Vol. 26, pp. 163–199). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C., & Jasnow, M. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development (Vol. 66). Boston: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joaquin, A. D. L. (2010a). Language, sociality, and the prefrontal cortex: Comparing child-adult and frontotemporal dementia-caregiver interactions. Discourse Studies, 12(4), 443–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joaquin, A. D. L. (2010b). The prefrontal cortex: Through education, socialization, and regression. In A. Mates, L. Mikesell, & M. Smith (Eds.), Language, interaction, and frontotemporal dementia: Reverse engineering the social mind (pp. 161–190). London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joaquin, A. D. L., & Schumann, J. (2013). Exploring the interactional instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joaquin, A. D. L. (2013). Enculturation processes in primary language acquisition. Sheffield: Equinox Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juszcyk, P., & Hohne, E. (1997). Infants’ memory for spoken words. Science, 277, 1984–1986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, W., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self: An event related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 785–794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemler Nelson, D., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Cassidy, K. W. (1989). How prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of Child Language, 16, 55–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2002). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development, 24(4), 372–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ko, E. S., Seidl, A., Cristia, A., Reimchen, M., & Soderstrom, M. (2015). Entrainment of prosody in the interaction of mothers with their young children. Journal of Child Language, 3, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. (2005a). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to the hedonic experience. Nature Reviews, 6, 691–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. (2005b). The orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. A. (2004). Early language acqusiition: Cracking the speech code. Nature, 5, 831–843. Infant Development, 37, 192–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P., Andruski, J., Chistovich, I., Chistovich, L., Kozhevnikova, E., Ryskina, V., Stolyarova, E., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277, 684–686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuvac-Kraljevic, J., Cepanec, M., & Simlesa, S. (2013). Gestural development and its relation to a child’s early vocabulary. Infant Behavior & Development, 37(2), 192–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, N., Mikesell, L., Joaquin, A. D. L., Mates, A., & Schumann, J. (2009). The interactional instinct: The evolution and acquisition of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, N., Mates, A. W., Joaquin, A. D. L., Mikesell, L., Schumann, J. (2011). The interactional instinct: The evolution and acquisition of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 424–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. P. A., Malcuit, G., & Feider, H. (1987). The development of infants’ responses to people and a doll: Implications for research in communication. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, H., Kuhl, P., & Tsao, F. (2003). An association between mother's speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science, 6(3), F1–F10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant- and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7, 209–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacFarlane, A. (1975). Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate. In R. Porter & M. O'Connor (Eds.), Parent-infant interaction. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mah, L., Arnold, M., & Grafman, J. (2004). Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1247–1255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G., Vijaya, S., Bandi Rao, S., & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old-infants. Science, 283, 77–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masataka, N. (1992). Pitch characteristics of Japanese maternal speech to infants. Journal of Child Language, 19(2), 213–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mates, A. W., & Joaquin, A. D. L. (2013). Affect and the brain. In J. Herschenbohn & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 417–435). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mates, A., Mikesell, L., & Smith, M. (Eds.). (2010). Language, interaction, and frontotemporal dementia: Reverse engineering the social mind. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D. (1985). Infants’ perception of facedness. In T. Field & N. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infancy (pp. 73–100). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., & Barrera, M. (1981). Infants' perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. Child Development, 52, 196–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R. I., & Nicoladis, E. (2000). Gesture reflects language development: Evidence from bilingual children. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 9(6), 190–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, P. B. (1991). Lessons from animal trainers: The effect of acoustic structure on an animal’s response. In P. Bateson & P. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology, volume 9: Human understanding and animal awareness (pp. 165–187). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGillion, M., Herbert, J., Pine, J., Vihman, M., dePaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Matthews, D. (2017). What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 88(1), 156–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., Kovack-Lesh, K. A., Goodwin, D., & McEchron, W. (2013). Infant-directed speech and the development of speech perception: Enhancing development or an unintended consequence? Cognition, 129, 362–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (2000). Language and gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, J., & Christophe, A. (1995). Maturation and learning of language in the first year of life. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences: A handbook for the field (pp. 943–954). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. Field & N. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–198). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E. (2006). From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates. Infant and Child Development, 15, 223–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E., & Molnar, P. (2004). Homo imitans or homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant behavior and development, 27, 54–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nonaka, A. (2004). The forgotten endagered languages: Lessons on the important of remembering from Thailand's ban Khor sign language. Language in Society, 33, 737–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K., Niyogi, P., Gray, S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, D., Xu, U., & Warren, S. F. (2010). Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 13354–13359.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Oshima-Takane, Y. (1988). Children learn from speech not addressed to them: The case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 15, 95–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oshima-Takane, Y. (1999). The learning of first- and second person pronouns in English. In R. Jackendoff, P. Bloom, & K. Wynn (Eds.), Language, logic, and concept: Essays in memory of John MacNamara (pp. 373–409). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth-order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Development, 67, 621–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pegg, J. E., Werker, J. F., & McLeod, P. J. (1992). Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 325–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popik, P., & Van Ree, J. M. (1992). Long-term facilitation of social recognition in rats by vasopressin related peptides: A structure-activity study. Life Sciences, 50(8), 567–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos: Man’s new dialogue with nature. London: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pye, C. (1986). Quiche Mayan speech to children. Journal of Child Language, 13, 85–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D., & Oakes, L. M. (Eds.). (2003). Early category and concept development: Making sense of blooming, buzzing confusion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns. Annual Review of Language Acquisition, 2, 85–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, N., Francis, B., Mason, J., & Lincoln, K. (2011). Do facial expressions develop before birth? PLoS One, 8(8), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Moore, L., Najafi, B., Dexter, A., Correa-Chávez, M., & Solís, J. (2007). Children’s development of cultural repertoires through participation in everyday routines and practices. In J. Gruseca & P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 490–515). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. (1998). The orbitofrontal cortex. In A. Roberts, T. W. Robbins, & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), The prefrontal cortex (pp. 67–86). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 353–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. (2001). Words in a sea of sounds: The output of infant statistical learning. Cognition, 81, 149–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J., & Thiessen, E. (2003). Pattern induction by infant language learners. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 484–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J., Aslin, R., & Newport, E. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schieffelin, B. (1990). The give and take of everyday life: Language socialization of Kaluli children. Tucson: Fenestra Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, J. H. (1999). A neurobiological basis for decision making in language pragmatics. Pragmatics & Cognition, 7(2), 283–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, R. G., & Terrell, B. Y. (1983). The role of input frequency in lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10, 57–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, B. (1969). The subjective estimate of relative word frequency. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 248–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimpi, P., Gamez, P., Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M. (2007). Syntactic priming in 3- and 4- year olf children: Evidence for abstract representations of transitive and dative forms. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1334–1346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, L., Nestor, S., Parikh, C., & Yull, A. (2009). Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy, 14, 654–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skolnick, D. and Fernald, A. (2003). Incidental word learning by two-year-olds, Poster presented at the Biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Tampa, FL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27, 501–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiner, J. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. In H. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 13, pp. 257–295). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storey, A., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2000). Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fathers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 79–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R., Gupta, S., Miller, K., Mills, S., & Orr, S. (2004). The effects of vasopressin in human facial responses related to social communication. Psychoneuro, 29, 35–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage based theory of language acqusition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S., Trainor, L. J., & Unyk, A. M. (1993). Music and speech processing in the first year of life. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, 24, 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1974). Conversations with a two-month-old. New Scientist, 62, 230–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. M. Bulowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321–347). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E., Als, H., & Adamson, L. (1979). Structure of early face-to-face communicative interactions. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech (pp. 349–370). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B. (1971). Recognition of memory. In H. Kendler & J. T. Spence (Eds.), Essays in Neobehaviorism (pp. 313–335). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M. (2014). Phonological development: The first two years. Boston: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling to speech: A re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language, 61, 397–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: Development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldrop, M. (1992). Complexity: The emerging science at the edge of order and chaos. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warlaumont, A. S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., & Oller, D. K. (2014). A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychological Science, 25, 1314–1324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–2152.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, O., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin administration to parent enhances infant physiological and behavioral readiness for social engagement. Biological Psychiatry, 72(12), 982–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J., & McLeod, P. J. (1989). Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed-talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responses. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 230–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G., Ironsmith, M., & Goldfein, M. (1974). Selective imitation of the passive construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17(2), 288–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna Dina L Joaquin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Joaquin, A.D.L. (2018). Language Acquisition. In: Shackelford, T., Weekes-Shackelford, V. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2416-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2416-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics