Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Ability and Willingness of Victim to Retaliate

Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1666-1

Synonyms

Definition

Action taken in return for an injury or offense.

Introduction

The concept of retaliation has historically been defined from both a behavioral and functional aspect. At its core, retaliation is based upon the premise of inciting organisms to increase benefit while reducing cost to oneself (McCullough et al. 2013). If a target organism can emit the potential ideal for retaliation toward an aggressor organism (typically in the form of retaliation itself), the target organism may increase its chances of lifetime productivity and may continue to evolve due to this willingness to retaliate. In other words, by making the potential costs of harm too high for an aggressor (imminent retaliation), the target organism is more likely to survive by avoiding harm against oneself.

Definitions that have previously influenced academics in the conceptualization of retaliation have typically been defined from a functional prospective. The O...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Adams, S. J. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–299). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  2. Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  3. Bixenstine, V. E., & Wilson, K. V. (1963). Effects of level of cooperative choice by the other player on choices in a prisoner’s dilemma game. Part II. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 139–147.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Parker, G. A. (1995). Punishment and animal societies. Nature, 373, 209–216.Google Scholar
  5. Diamond, S. R. (1977). The effect of fear on the aggressive responses of anger aroused and revenge motivated subjects. Journal of Psychology, 95, 185–188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Figueredo, A. J. (1995). Preliminary report: Family deterrence of domestic violence in Spain. Tucson: Department of Psychology, University of Arizona.Google Scholar
  7. Govier, T. (2002). Forgiveness and revenge. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  8. Hayashi, N., Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Yamagishi, T. (1999). Reciprocity, trust, and the sense of control: Across-societal study. Rationality and Society, 11, 27–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kim, S. H., & Smith, R. H., (1993). Revenge and conflict escalation. Negotiation Journal, 9, 37–43.Google Scholar
  10. Kim, S. H., Smith, R. H., & Brigham, N. L. (1998). Effects of power imbalance and the presence of third parties on reactions to harm: Upward and downward revenge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 353–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. McCullough, M. E., Kurzban, R., & Tabak, B. A. (2008). Evolved mechanisms for revenge and forgiveness. In P. R. Shaver and M. Milulincer (Eds.), Understanding and reducing aggression, violence, and their consequences (221–238). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  12. McCullough, M. E., Kurzban, R., & Tabak, B. A. (2013). Cognitive systems for revenge and forgiveness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 1–58. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11002160.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Schumann, K., & Ross, M. (2010). The benefits, costs, and paradox of revenge. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(12), 1193–1205. doi:10.1111/j.17519004.2010.00322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Uniacke, S. (2000). Why is revenge wrong? The Journal of Value Inquiry, 34, 61–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Florida State UniversityTallahasseeUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Kevin Beaver
    • 1
  1. 1.Florida State UniversityTallahasseeUSA