Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Holmes and Sherman (1982) on Ground Squirrels

  • Ferenc KocsorEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1497-1

Keywords

Genetic Relatedness Ground Squirrel Inclusive Fitness Unrelated Female Arctic Ground Squirrel 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Synonyms

Definition

Phenotype matching refers to the ability to detect genetic relatedness based on phenotypic cues of the self or close kin.

Introduction

In laboratory studies it was found that Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi) are able to recognize kin with which they did not share natal burrows. Related female individuals, even if they were unfamiliar to each other, were less agonistic than unrelated individuals. Field studies also showed that littermate full-sisters treat each other differently than maternal half-sisters. This suggests that both rearing environment and relatedness have crucial contribution to nepotistic behavior in ground squirrels. Similar results with other rodent species imply that learning and recognition of cues of probable genetic relatedness, known as phenotype matching, might be a mechanism which influences social interactions.

Mechanisms of Kin Recognition

Hamilton’s inclusive fitness theory suggests that it would be advantageous for individuals to show a biased behavior for conspecifics who are genetically more closely related. Preference for kin could help optimize prosocial behavior within the group (nepotism) and mate choice decisions (inbreeding avoidance, optimal outbreeding, see Bateson 1983). However, for nonsedentary species, as dispersal from the native group is common, differentiating between kin and nonkin could be a challenge. At least four possible mechanisms of kin recognition were outlined (Mateo 2003).
  • First, spatial cues (i.e., co-residence) can inform individuals about relatedness.

  • Second, early association is a somewhat better heuristic for the estimation of kinship. For species which disperse after the juvenile period and, therefore, may easily find themselves in the proximity of nonrelated conspecifics, co-residence is an uncertain cue of genetic closeness.

  • Third, as a theoretical possibility, it has been proposed that recognition alleles could serve the recognition of related individuals. This allele should be expressed phenotypically, recognized by others and it is expected to increase the likelihood of preferential treatment of the individuals with that cue.

  • Fourth, if there is a correlation between the similarity of phenotypic traits and the amount of shared genes, phenotype matching can provide individuals with the necessary information about relatedness. During the juvenile period they can learn either the cues (e.g., odors) of their littermates and other co-residing conspecifics (other-referent phenotype matching), or that of their own (self-referent phenotype matching), and later use these as a recognition template to assess genetic relatedness to unfamiliar individuals.

Ground Squirrels as Model Animals

Holmes and Sherman (1982) used Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi) and Arctic ground squirrels (S. parryii) as model animals to test whether phenotype matching is used for kin recognition in these species. They cross-fostered preweaned pups in a laboratory environment and tested the behavior of related and familiar individuals and related and unfamiliar individuals in pairs. It turned out that ground squirrels which were reared together treat each other as siblings, that is, they are not more agonistic than sibling pairs. Besides, female siblings reared apart showed less aggressiveness than unrelated female pairs. Field observations of Belding’s ground squirrels revealed also females’ ability to discriminate between full-sisters and maternal half-sisters, despite they shared the natal burrow with both sibling types. They are more cooperative and less agonistic with full-sisters than with half-sisters – an impressive demonstration of the inclusive fitness theory. Hence, it was suggested, shared environment shape prosocial behavior and so does genetic relatedness.

In subsequent experiments, the role of phenotype matching in kin recognition was studied further. It was shown that females of S. beldingi treat paternal half-sisters differently than unrelated females (Holmes 1986a). In this case, as these siblings were reared apart, familiarity could not contribute to the biased behavior. When female ground squirrels were cross-fostered and reared in a mixed group of related and unrelated littermates, paired arena tests confirmed that both shared natal environment and relatedness affects the frequency of agonistic acts (Holmes 1986b). On the one hand, when females encountered unrelated females, the level of aggression was modest. Note that due to cross-fostering they were mutually the sisters of each other’s littermates. This shows that exposure to a certain phenotype in the natal burrow is used later as a template to which other individuals are compared (Holmes 1984). In this case, information about phenotypic cues of nonrelated littermates is used to recognize kin of that littermates (other-referent phenotype matching). On the other hand, unfamiliar sisters are recognized and preferred as well, even in case the pup does not share its natal environment with any relatives, preventing the use of phenotypic cues other than her own (self-referent phenotype matching).

Conclusion

The ability to recognize kin, and to prefer them over conspecifics who are genetically not related, has been observed in many taxa, including insects and mammals. It has been suggested that this bias was selected for during evolution and might be best explained by the theory of inclusive fitness. Several mechanisms have been put forward to account for kin recognition, most notably phenotype matching. In their influential paper, Holmes and Sherman (1982) provided evidence that ground squirrel pups (Spermophilus sp.) learn phenotypic cues of both their littermates and their own, to which unfamiliar individuals are compared at later encounters. Though theoretically both sexes might use this ability the discriminate between conspecifics with different degrees of relatedness, biased behavior is limited to female–female interactions. This suggests that kin-recognition is used in these rodent species as a means of nepotism, rather than that of optimal mate choice.

Cross-References

References

  1. Bateson, P. P. G. (1983). Optimal outbreeding. In P. P. G. Bateson (Ed.), Mate choice (pp. 257–277). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Holmes, W. G. (1984). Sibling recognition in thirteen-lined ground squirrels: Effects of genetic relatedness, rearing association, and olfaction. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 14(3), 225–233. doi:10.1007/BF00299622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Holmes, W. G. (1986a). Identification of paternal half-siblings by captive Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 34(2), 321–327. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80099-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Holmes, W. G. (1986b). Kin recognition by phenotype matching in female Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 34(Part 1), 38–47. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(86)90004-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Holmes, W. G., & Sherman, P. W. (1982). The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels. American Zoologist, 22(3), 491–517. doi:10.1093/icb/22.3.491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Mateo, J. M. (2003). Kin recognition in ground squirrels and other rodents. Journal of Mammalogy, 84(4), 1163–1181. doi:10.1644/BLe-011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of PsychologyUniversity of PecsPecsHungary