Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Height and Dominance

  • Thomas V. PolletEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1420-1

Keywords

Body Size Leadership Quality Narrow Passage Hierarchical Position Tall Individual 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Synonyms

Definition

The relationship between height and dominance refers to a position in a hierarchy based on body height.

Introduction

In many mammalian species, dominance is positively associated with body size. All else being equal, larger individuals should be more likely to win than smaller individuals in contests. Building on findings from the animal kingdom, evolutionary psychologists have argued for similar associations between body size and dominance in humans. Indeed, it seems that both historically and cross-culturally body size is believed positively associated with dominance. One often noted example is that political leaders in Melanesia and Polynesia are referred to as “big men,” and they are often physically big men (Sahlins 1963). Similarly, today, in Western societies, positive associations between stature and political leadership exist. For example, ratings of American presidents’ leadership abilities positively correlate with their stature. Intuitively, one would therefore speculate that across the globe, dominance positively relates to height. In this encyclopedia entry, one aspect of body size, height, is discussed, and the evidence for a positive association between stature and interpersonal dominance is briefly reviewed.

Height and Dominance

Dominance interactions in humans remain understudied, and evidence for a positive relationship between height and dominance thus remains rather circumstantial. For example, research suggests that taller individuals might have more strength (Vaz et al. 2002), have better fighting ability (von Rueden et al. 2008), and are able to generate a larger force when striking (Carrier 2011) than shorter individuals. Taller individuals are also perceived as more powerful and stronger than shorter individuals (e.g., Huang et al. 2002). The reverse also holds that more powerful individuals are estimated as being taller (e.g., Fessler et al. 2012). Tallness also is found to be positively associated with perceived leadership qualities (Stulp et al. 2013). Taller individuals might have more authority in business settings (Judge and Cable 2004) or sports (Stulp et al. 2012). Finally, only a few studies have studied actual behavior which can be labeled as dominant in naturalistic settings, such as having “right of way” (Stulp et al. 2015). For example, when faced with a narrow passage, taller individuals are more likely to take precedence than shorter individuals, all else being equal.

Conclusion

In conclusion, various strands of evidence corroborate the claim that tallness is positively associated with dominance. Yet, it is important to bear in mind that height is clearly correlated to a whole suite of other traits, including socioeconomic status, and as such teasing apart the independent effect of size itself for dominance could be difficult (especially given that we cannot experimentally manipulate human height directly). Moreover, it should be noted that any relationships found between height and interpersonal dominance tend to be weak. Perhaps, unsurprisingly muscularity appears to be a better correlate of interpersonal dominance, at least in perception studies (e.g., Sell et al. 2012). Finally, it must be noted that there is actually not that much research on height and dominance and that it is mostly limited to men. Therefore, while Western societies might have fewer dominance interactions due to the state monopoly of violence, height and dominance remain an interesting domain for further investigation, but studying the role of stature in dominant behavior in a naturalistic setting might prove challenging.

Cross-References

References and Further Reading

  1. Carrier, D. R. (2011). The advantage of standing up to fight and the evolution of habitual bipedalism in hominins. PLoS One, 6(5), e19630.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019630 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Fessler, D. M. T., Holbrook, C., & Snyder, J. K. (2012). Weapons make the man (larger): Formidability is represented as size and strength in humans. PloS One, 7(4), e32751 . https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032751.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Huang, W., Olson, J. S., & Olson, G. M. (2002). Camera angle effects dominance in video-mediated communication. Proceedings of the conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 716–717).Google Scholar
  4. Judge, T. A., & Cable, D. M. (2004). The effect of physical height on workplace success and income: Preliminary test of a theoretical model. J Appl Psychol, 89(3), 428–441.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Sahlins, M. D. (1963). Poor man, rich man, big-man, chief: Political types in Melanesia and Polynesia. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 5(3), 285–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Sell, A., Hone, L. S. E., & Pound, N. (2012). The importance of physical strength to human males. Human Nature, 23(1), 30–44.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Stulp, G., Buunk, A. P., Verhulst, S., & Pollet, T. V. (2012). High and mighty: Height increases authority in professional refereeing. Evol Psychol, 10(3), 588–601.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Stulp, G., Buunk, A. P., Verhulst, S., & Pollet, T. V. (2013). Tall claims? Sense and nonsense about the importance of height of US presidents. Leadersh Q, 24(1), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Stulp, G., Buunk, A. P., Verhulst, S., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Human height is positively related to interpersonal dominance in dyadic interactions. PLOS ONE, 10(2), e0117860. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117860.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Vaz, M., Hunsberger, S., & Diffey, B. (2002). Prediction equations for handgrip strength in healthy Indian male and female subjects encompassing a wide age range. Annals of Human Biology, 29(2), 131–141 . https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460110058962.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2008). The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. Evol Hum Behav, 29(6), 402–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.VU University AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands

Section editors and affiliations

  • Minna Lyons
    • 1
  1. 1.University of LiverpoolLiverpoolUK