Encyclopedia of the History of Psychological Theories

2012 Edition
| Editors: Robert W. Rieber

Theories of Memory, History of

  • David J. Murray
Reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0463-8_14

Introduction

In attempting to formulate a history of theories of memory, it must be noted that the English word “memory” itself has a broadness of application that is not paralleled in other languages. In French, the words la mémoire refer to the ability that the mind–brain system has for retaining representations both of internal events and of external reality; and the words un souvenir refer to an individual retained representation generally identified by its content (referent). For example, if one wanted to translate the question, “Do insects have memory?” into French, one would use la mémoire as an equivalent for the English word “memory.” But if one wanted to translate the question “How strong is your memory of your first day at school?” one would use le souvenir. An analogous classification is found in German, where memory-the-ability is translated as das Gedächtnis and an individually experienced memory representation is translated as eine Erinnerung.

These observations lead to...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access

References

  1. Abercrombie, J. (1843). Inquiries concerning the intellectual powers, and the investigation of truth. Boson: Otis, Broaders, and Company.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1974). Human associative memory. Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
  3. Andrade, J. (Ed.), (2001). Working memory in perspective. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  4. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human development: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  5. Baars, B. J. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  7. Baddeley, A. D. (2006). Working memory: An overview. In S. J. Pickering (Ed.), Working memory and education (pp. 1–13). London: Academic Press, Elsevier.Google Scholar
  8. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  9. Baddeley, A. D., & Larsen, J. D. (2007a). The phonological loop unmasked? A comment on the evidence for a “perceptual-gestural” alternative. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 497–504.Google Scholar
  10. Baddeley, A. D., & Larsen, J. D. (2007b). The phonological loop: Some answers and some questions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 512–518.Google Scholar
  11. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experiemental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Berman, D. E. (2003). Taste aversion and preference learning in animals. In J. H. Byrne (Ed.), Learning and memory (2nd ed., pp. 645–647). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.Google Scholar
  13. Bernbach, H. A. (1965). A forgetting model for paired associate learning. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 2, 128–144.Google Scholar
  14. Blankenship, A. B. (1938). Memory span: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 35, 1–25.Google Scholar
  15. Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to behaviorism. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Boudewijnse, G.-J. A., Murray, D. J., & Bandomir, C. A. (1999). Herbart’s mathematical psychology. History of Psychology, 2, 163–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Boudewijnse, G.-J. A., Murray, D. J., & Bandomir, C. A. (2001). The fate of Herbart’s mathematical psychology. History of Psychology, 4, 107–132.Google Scholar
  18. Bower, G. H. (1961). Application of a model to paired associate learning. Psychometrika, 25, 255–280.Google Scholar
  19. Bower, G. H. (1972). A selective review of organizational factors in memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 93–137). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  20. Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1981). Recency effects in visual memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 1–15.Google Scholar
  21. Brown, G. D. A., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review, 114, 539–576.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107, 127–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Bush, R. R., & Mosteller, F. (1955). Stochastic models for learning. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Caplan, H. (Trans.) (1954). Rhetorica ad Herennium. The Loeb Library, No. 403. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
  25. Ceraso, J. (1967). The interference theory of forgetting. Scientific American, 317(October), 117–124.Google Scholar
  26. Coombs, C. H., Dawes, R. M., & Tversky, A. (1970). Mathematical psychology: An elementary introduction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc.Google Scholar
  27. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Crowder, R. G. (2003). Eidetic imagery. In J. H. Byrne (Ed.), Learning and memory (2nd ed., pp. 130–132). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.Google Scholar
  29. Dalton, P., Santangelo, V., & Spence, S. (2009). The role of working memory in auditory selective attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 2126–2132.Google Scholar
  30. Danziger, K. (2008). Making the mind: A history of memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Davis, M., & Egger, M. D. (2003). Habituation and sensitization in vertebrates. In J. H. Byrne (Ed.), Learning and memory (2nd ed., pp. 223–226). New York: MacMillan Reference USA.Google Scholar
  32. Ebbinghaus, H. (1964). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1885).Google Scholar
  33. Edgell, B. (1924). Theories of memory. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  34. Estes, W. K. (1950). Towards a statistical theory of learning. Psychological Review, 47, 96–107.Google Scholar
  35. Estes, W. K. (1960). Learning theory and the new “mental chemistry”. Psychological Review, 67, 202–223.Google Scholar
  36. Estes, W. K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organization in memory. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 161–190). Washington, DC: V. H. Winston & Sons.Google Scholar
  37. Estes, W. K. (1997). Processes of memory loss, recovery, and distortion. Psychological Review, 104, 148–169.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Estes, W. K. (2003). Mathematical learning theory. In J. H. Byrne (Ed.), Learning and memory (2nd ed., pp. 359–362). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.Google Scholar
  39. Estes, W. K., Hopkins, B. L., & Crothers, E. J. (1960). All-or-none and conservation effects in the learning and retention of paired associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 329–339.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Farah, M. J., Hammond, K., Levine, D., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imaging: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439–462.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Foucault, M. (1973). Madness and civilization: A history of insanity in the age of reason. New York: Vintage Books. Original work published 1965.Google Scholar
  42. Gaito, J. (1966). Molecular psychobiology: A chemical approach to learning and other behavior. Springfield, IL: Thomas.Google Scholar
  43. Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  44. Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 5, 121–122.Google Scholar
  45. Garden, S., Cormoldi, C., & Logie, R. H. (2002). Visuo-spatial working memory in navigation. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 35–50.Google Scholar
  46. Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 351–360.Google Scholar
  48. Glanzer, M., Adams, J. K., Iverson, G. J., & Kim, K. (1993). The regularities of recognition memory. Psychological Review, 104, 728–748.Google Scholar
  49. Gomulicki, B. R. (1953). The development and present status of the trace theory of memory. British Journal of Psychology Monograph Supplements XXIX. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Grey, R. (1859). Memoria technica, or method of artificial memory. Oxford: J. Vincent. Original work published 1730.Google Scholar
  51. Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 1–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Gulliksen, H. (1934). A rational equation of the learning curve based on Thorndike’s law of effect. Journal of General Psychology, 11, 395–434.Google Scholar
  53. Haber, R. N., & Haber, R. B. (1964). Eidetic imagery: I. Frequency. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19, 131–138.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Hacking, I. (1995). Rewriting the soul. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Hamilton, S. W. (1852). The works of Thomas Reid, D. D (3rd ed.). Edinburgh: MacLachlan and Stewart.Google Scholar
  56. Hanley, J. R., & Cohen, G. (2008). Memory for people: Faces, names and voices. In G. Cohen & M. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the real world (3rd ed., pp. 107–140). New York: Psychology Press Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  57. Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. (2 Vols.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Hebb, D. O., & Foord, E. N. (1945). Errors of visual recognition and the nature of the trace. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 335–348.Google Scholar
  59. Herbart, J. F. (1890) Psychologie als Wissenschaft [Psychology as science]. In K. Kehrbach & O. Flügel (Eds.), Jon. Fr. Herbart’s sämtliche Werke in chronologischer Reihenfolge (Part I: Vol. 5, pp. 177–434). Langensalza, Germany: Hermann Beyer und Söhne. (Original work published 1824).Google Scholar
  60. Hilgard, E. R., & Campbell, A. A. (1936). The course of acquisition and retention of conditioned eyelid responses in man. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 227–247.Google Scholar
  61. Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 16, 96–101.Google Scholar
  62. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc.Google Scholar
  63. Hull, C. L., Hovland, C. I., Ross, R. T., Hall, M., Perkins, D. T., & Fitch, F. B. (1940). Mathematico-deductive theory of rote learning. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  64. Hume, D. (1955). A treatise of human nature. (L. A. Selby-Bigge, Ed.). London: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1739).Google Scholar
  65. Iacoboni, M. (2009). Mirroring people: The science of empathy and how we connect with others. New York: Picador.Google Scholar
  66. Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on “prehension”. Mind, 12, 75–79.Google Scholar
  67. James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover. Original work published 1890.Google Scholar
  68. Jevons, W. G. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination. Nature, 3, 281–282.Google Scholar
  69. John, E. R. (1967). Mechanisms of memory. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  70. Jones, D. M., Hughes, R. W., & Macken, W. J. (2006). Perceptual organization masquerading as phonological storage: Further support for a perceptual-gestural view of short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 265–281.Google Scholar
  71. Jones, D. M., Hughes, R. W., & Macken, W. J. (2007). The phonological store abandoned. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 505–511.Google Scholar
  72. Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2007). Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon. Psychological Review, 114, 1–37.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Kamil, A. C., Balda, A. P., & Olso, D. J. (1994). Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial-arm maze. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 385–393.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Kandel, E. R. (2006). In search of memory: The emergence of a new science of mind. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
  75. King, D. B., & Wertheimer, M. (2005). Max Wertheimer and Gestalt theory. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  76. Köhler, W., & von Restorff, H. (1937). II. Analyse von Vorgängen im Spurenfeld. Zur Theorie der Reproduktion. [II. Analysis of processes in the trace-field. Towards a theory of memory reproduction]. Psychologische Forschung, 21, 56–112.Google Scholar
  77. Laming, D. (1973). Mathematical psychology. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  78. Laming, D. (2009). Failure to recall. Psychological Review, 116, 157–186.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain mechanisms and intelligence. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  80. Lashley, K. S. (1960). In search of the engram. In F. A. Beach, D. O. Hebb, C. T. Morgan, & H. W. Nissen (Eds.), The neuropsychology of Lashley (pp. 478–505). New York: McGraw-Hill. Original work published 1950.Google Scholar
  81. Levine, D. N., Warrach, J., & Farah, M. (1985). Two visual systems in mental imagery: Dissociation of “what” and “where” in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions. Neurology, 35, 1010–1018.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Lewandowsky, S., Duncan, M., & Brown, G. D. A. (2004). Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 771–790.Google Scholar
  83. Lones, T. E. (1912). Aristotle’s researches in natural science. London: West, Newman.Google Scholar
  84. Marks, D. F. (1999). Consciousness, mental imagery and action. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 567–585.Google Scholar
  85. Martin, E. (1965). Transfer of verbal paired associates. Psychological Review, 72, 327–343.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. McKeon, R. (Ed.), (1941). De memoria et reminiscentia. In The basic works of Aristotle (pp. 607–617). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  87. Melton, A. W., & von Lackum, W. J. (1941). Retroactive and proactive inhibition in retention: Evidence for a two-factor theory of retroactive inhibition. American Journal of Psychology, 54, 157–173.Google Scholar
  88. Mensink, G.-J., & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model for interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434–455.Google Scholar
  89. Metcalfe, J. (1991). Composite memories. In W. E. Hockley & S. Lewandowsky (Eds.), Relating theory and data: Essays on human memory in honor of Bennet B. Murdock (pp. 399–423). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  90. Mills, W. (1898). The nature and development of animal intelligence. London: T. Fisher Unwin.Google Scholar
  91. Morgan, C. L. (1895). An introduction to comparative psychology. London: Walter Scott Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  92. Moulin, C. J. A., & Gathercole, E. (2008). Memory changes across the lifespan. In G. Cohen & M. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the real world (3rd ed., pp. 305–326). New York: Psychology Press Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
  93. Müller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Untersuchung des Gedächtnisses. [Experimental contributions to the investigation of memory]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, Ergänzungsband, No. 1.Google Scholar
  94. Murdock, B. B. Jr., (1983). A distributed memory model for serial-order information. Psychological Review, 90, 316–338.Google Scholar
  95. Murdock, B. B. Jr., (1993). TODAM2: A model for the storage and retrieval of item, associative, and serial-order information. Psychological Review, 100, 183–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Murray, D. J. (1967). The role of speech responses in short-term memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 21, 263–276.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Murray, D. J. (1988). A history of Western psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  98. Murray, D. J. (1994). What are the “goals” of the human memory system? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 676–677.Google Scholar
  99. Murray, D. J. (1995). Gestalt psychology and the cognitive revolution. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
  100. Murray, D. J. (2000). The trace deletion hypothesis in relation to partial matching theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 43–44.Google Scholar
  101. Murray, D. J., & Bandomir, C. A. (2002). The predictive validity of Herbart’s psychology. Psychologie et Histoire, 3, 1–27.Google Scholar
  102. Murray, D. J., & Farahmand, B. (1998). Gestalt psychology and evolutionary psychology. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 254–287). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  103. Murray, D. J., & Newman, F. M. (1973). Visual and verbal coding in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 58–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Murray, D. J., & Ross, H. E. (1982). Vives (1538) on memory and recall. Canadian Psychology, 23, 22–31.Google Scholar
  105. Murray, D. J., Mastronardi, J., & Duncan, S. (1972). Selective attention to ‘physical’ versus ‘verbal’ aspects of coloured words. Psychonomic Science, 26, 305–307.Google Scholar
  106. Murray, D. J., Boudreau, N., Burggraf, K. K., Dobell, L., Guger, S. L., Leask, A., et al. (1999). A grouping interpretation of the modality effect in immediate probed recognition. Memory & Cognition, 27, 234–245.Google Scholar
  107. Murray, D. J., Burhop, J., Centa, S., Chande, N., Oinonen, K., Thomas, T., et al. (1998). A partial matching theory of the mirror effect in immediate probed recognition. Memory & Cognition, 26, 1196–1213.Google Scholar
  108. Nairne, J. S. (1990). A feature model of immediate memory. Memory & Cognition, 16, 343–352.Google Scholar
  109. Neath, I., & Nairne, J. S. (1995). Word-length effects in immediate memory: Overwriting trace decay theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 429–441.Google Scholar
  110. Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2003). Human memory (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
  111. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  112. Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  113. Nicolas, S., & Murray, D. J. (1999). Théodule Ribot (1839–1916), founder of French psychology: A biographical introduction. History of Psychology, 2, 271–301.Google Scholar
  114. Norman, D. A. (Ed.). (1970). Models of human memory. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  115. Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Forgetting in immediate serial recall: Decay, temporal distinctiveness, or interference? Psychological Review, 115, 544–576.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, and Winston Inc.Google Scholar
  117. Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  118. Paivio, A. (2007). Mind and its evolution: A dual coding theoretical approach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  119. Pavlov, I. P. (1960). Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1927).Google Scholar
  120. Penney, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 17, 398–422.Google Scholar
  121. Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (2002). The self-organizing consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297–388.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library.Google Scholar
  123. Postman, L. (1971). Organization and interference. Psychological Review, 78, 290–302.Google Scholar
  124. Postman, L., & Underwood, B. J. (1973). Critical issues in interference theory. Memory & Cognition, 1, 19–40.Google Scholar
  125. Pribram, K. H. (1969). The neurophysiology of remembering. Scientific American, 220, 73–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93–134.Google Scholar
  127. Ratcliff, R. (1990). Connectionist models of recognition memory: Constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions. Psychological Review, 97, 285–308.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  128. Ratcliff, R., & Starns, J. J. (2009). Modeling confidence and response time in recognition memory. Psychological Research, 116, 59–83.Google Scholar
  129. Restle, F., & Greeno, J. G. (1970). Introduction to mathematical psychology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  130. Riley, D. A. (1962). Memory for form. In L. Postman (Ed.), Psychology in the making: Histories of selected research problems (pp. 402–465). New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
  131. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Rock, I. (1957). The role of repetition in associative learning. American Journal of Psychology, 70, 186–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Roediger, H. L., III. (1980). Memory metaphors in cognitive psychology. Memory & Cognition, 8, 231–246.Google Scholar
  134. Romanes, G. J. (1882). Animal intelligence. London: Kegan Paul, Trench & Co.Google Scholar
  135. Romanes, G. J. (1883). Mental evolution in animals. London: Kegan Paul, Trench & Co.Google Scholar
  136. Romanes, G. J. (1888). Mental evolution in man. London: Kegan Paul, Trench & Co.Google Scholar
  137. Russell, W. R. (1959). Brain memory learning: A neurologist’s view. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  138. Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurological and Neurosurgical Psychiatry, 20, 11–12.Google Scholar
  139. Searleman, A., & Herrmann, D. (1994). Memory from a broader perspective. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  140. Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. (1982). Mental images and their transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  141. Spillane, J. D. (1981). The doctrine of the nerves: Chapters in the history of neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  142. Standing, L. (1973). Learning 10, 000 pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 207–222.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  143. Tan, L., & Ward, G. (2000). A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1589–1625.Google Scholar
  144. Thurstone, L. L. (1930). The learning function. Journal of General Psychology, 3, 469–493.Google Scholar
  145. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 382–403). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  146. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  147. von Békésy, G. (1967). Sensory inhibition. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  148. von Restorff, H. (1933). Über die Wirkung von Bereichsbildung im Spurenfeld. [On the effect of the formation of group domains in the trace-field]. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 299–342.Google Scholar
  149. Watkins, M. J. (2003). Memory span. In J. H. Byrne (Editor-in-Chief), Learning and memory (2nd ed.) (pp. 379–381). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.Google Scholar
  150. Watson, J. B. (1928). The ways of behaviorism. New York: Harpers.Google Scholar
  151. Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism (Vol. 2). Chicago: Chicago University Press. Original work published 1924.Google Scholar
  152. Waugh, N. C., & Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory. Psychological Review, 72, 89–104.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Wickelgren, W. A., & Norman, D. A. (1966). Strength models and serial position in short-term recognition memory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 3, 316–347.Google Scholar
  154. Williams, H., Conway, M., & Cohen, G. (2008). Autobiographical memory. In G. Cohen & M. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the real world (3rd ed., pp. 21–90). New York: Psychology Press Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  155. Wolfson, H. A. (1935). The internal senses in Latin, Arabic and Hebrew philosophic texts. Harvard Theological Review, 28, 69–133.Google Scholar
  156. Woodworth, R. S. (1938). Experimental psychology. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
  157. Wright, D., & Loftus, E. (2008). Eyewitness memory. In G. Cohen & M. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the real world (3rd ed., pp. 91–105). New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  158. Wulf, F. (1922). Über die Veränderung von Vorstellungen (Gedächtnis und Gestalt). [On the transformation of Vorstellungen (memory and Gestalt)]. Psychologische Forschung, 1, 333–373. Abridged translation in W. D. Ellis (Ed.) (1938), A source book of Gestalt psychology (pp. 136–148). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  159. Wundt, W. (1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. [Principles of physiological psychology]. Leipzig: W. Engelmann.Google Scholar
  160. Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  161. Young, M. N. (1961). Bibliography of memory. Philadelphia: Chilton.Google Scholar
  162. Zeki, S. (1992). The visual image in mind and brain. Scientific American, 267, 68–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • David J. Murray
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyQueen’s UniversityKingstonCanada