Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Escape Response

Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_270-1

Synonyms

Definition

An escape response is a coordinated response by an animal to end exposure to a noxious stimulus or potentially dangerous experience. Escape responses may be innate or learned and function to improve the survival of the animal.

Introduction

An escape response functions to terminate a negative experience or exposure to a hazard. It will take a form likely to produce this outcome, which may be fleeing, hiding, or releasing materials. For example, a marine mollusk such as an octopus may release ink (Derby 2007), or an insect such as an aphid may drop from a branch (Gish and Inbar 2006). Many animals will flee rapidly from a threat along an unpredictable trajectory or zigzag path. Predator escape is one of the most commonly studied escape responses; however, it may be shown in response to a range of stimuli such as temperature extremes, a source of pain, or any sudden intense stimulus.

Escape responses with a known and predictable form may...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious behavior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 800.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Derby, C. D. (2007). Escape by inking and secreting: Marine molluscs avoid predators through a rich array of chemicals and mechanisms. The Biological Bulletin, 213, 274–289.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Eaton, R. C., & Emberley, D. S. (1991). How stimulus direction determines the trajectory of the Mauthner-initiated escape response in a teleost fish. Journal of Experimental Biology, 161, 469–487.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Faulkes, Z. (2015). Motor neurons in the escape response circuit of white shrimp (Litopenaeus setiferus). PeerJ, 3, e1112.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Gish, M., & Inbar, M. (2006). Host location by apterous aphids after escape dropping from the plant. Journal of Insect Behavior, 19, 143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Griffin, A. S., Blumstein, D. T., & Evans, C. S. (2000). Training captive-bred or translocated animals to avoid predators. Conservation Biology, 14, 1317–1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Heitler, W. J., Fraser, K., & Ferrero, E. A. (2000). Escape behaviour in the stomatopod crustacean Squilla mantis, and the evolution of the caridoid escape reaction. Journal of Experimental Biology, 203, 183–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Herbert-Read, J. E., Buhl, J., Hu, F., Ward, A. J., & Sumpter, D. J. (2015). Initiation and spread of escape waves within animal groups. Royal Society Open Science, 2, 140355.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Holland, C. V., & Cox, D. M. (2001). Toxocara in the mouse: A model for parasite-altered host behaviour? Journal of Helminthology, 75, 125–135.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Lewis, R. S., & Hurst, J. L. (2004). The assessment of bar chewing as an escape behavior in laboratory mice. Animal Welfare, 13, 19–25.Google Scholar
  11. Libersat, F., & Moore, J. (2000). The parasite Moniliformis moniliformis alters the escape response of its cockroach host Periplaneta americana. Journal of Insect Behavior, 13, 103–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Maier, S. F. (1984). Learned helplessness and animal models of depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 8, 435–446.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Animal Welfare DivisionAmerican Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA)SchaumburgUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Emily Patterson-Kane
    • 1
  1. 1.Animal Welfare DivisionAmerican Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA)SchaumburgUSA