Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

The Comparator

  • Jessica X. Brooks
  • Kathleen E. CullenEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_217-1

Definition

The theoretical concept of a “comparator” has been proposed in order to explain how the brain distinguishes sensory information that is expected based on our own voluntary behaviors versus that generated by events in the external world. Specifically, the comparator is thought to subtract the motor-based estimate of sensory inflow from actual sensory signal to suppress the representation of expected stimuli. This self-monitoring mechanism ensures accurate motor control and stable perception.

Overview

It is important to correctly detect our own actions in order to effectively interact with and build an accurate representation of our environment. However, our sensory systems are activated by both external events (sensory exafference) and our own actions (sensory reafference). The prevailing view is that the brain largely achieves control of posture and accurate perceptual representation of the environment during voluntary actions by suppressing self-generated sensory...
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bell, C. C., Han, V., & Sawtell, N. B. (2008). Cerebellum-like structures and their implications for cerebellar function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of self produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 635–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brooks, J. X., & Cullen, K. E. (2014). Early vestibular processing does not discriminate active from passive self-motion if there is a discrepancy between predicted and actual proprioceptive feedback. Journal of Neurophysiology, 111(12), 2465–2478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brooks, J. X., Carriot, J., & Cullen, K. E. (2015). Learning to expect the unexpected: Rapid updating in primate cerebellum during voluntary self-motion. Nature Neuroscience, 18(9), 1310–1317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Couchman, J. J. (2015). Humans and monkeys distinguish between self-generated, opposing, and random actions. Animal Cognition, 18(1), 231–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cullen, K. E. (2011). The neural encoding of self-motion. Current opinion in neurobiology, 21(4), 587–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 18(4), 196–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Sommer, M. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2008). Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 317–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. von Holst, E., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Wolpert, D. M., & Miall, R. C. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks, 9(8), 1265–1279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhysiologyMcGill UniversityMontrealCanada
  2. 2.Biomedical EngineeringThe Johns Hopkins UniversityBaltimoreUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Joseph Boomer
    • 1
  1. 1.University at BuffaloBuffaloUSA