Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Canine Communication

  • Irena PetakEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1695-1

Synonyms

Definition

Dogs may communicate with conspecifics, humans, and other domestic animals they live with. When they communicate, they use visual, auditory, olfactory, and tactile signals. Domestication has greatly changed dogs’ physical appearance, and therefore, visual communication may not be equally important for dogs as for their wild relatives. Whereas some messages may be important for recognition and signaling in close proximity, others are broadcasted for long-distance communication.

Introduction

Between the carnivores, dogs and their ancestor, wolves, can be considered as the most social. Therefore, they develop an effective communication system with an abundant number of signals to maintain cohesion of the group, no matter if that is wolf pack, human family, or a group of feral dogs (e.g., Bradshaw and Nott 1995; Feddersen-Petersen 2007). For communication and recognition in close proximity, they can use body language, body odor, touch, and certain...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Adams, G. J., & Johnson, K. G. (1994). Behavioural responses to barking and other auditory stimuli during night-time sleeping and waking in the domestic dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 39, 151–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beaver, B. V. (2009). Canine behavior: Insights and answers (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders – Elsevier.Google Scholar
  3. Bekoff, M. (1977). Social communication in canids: Evidence for the evolution of a stereotyped mammalian display. Science, 197, 1097–1099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bekoff, M. (1979). Ground scratching by male domestic dogs: A composite signal. Journal of Mammalogy, 60, 847–848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bekoff, M. (2001). Observations of scent-marking and discriminating self from others by a domestic dog (Canis familiaris): Tales of displaced yellow snow. Behavioural Processes, 55, 75–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boitani, L., & Ciucci, P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 7, 49–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bradshaw, J. W. S., & Nott, H. M. R. (1995). Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs. In J. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 115–130). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bubna-Littitz, H. (2007). Sensory physiology and dog behaviour. In P. Jensen (Ed.), The behavioural biology of dogs (pp. 105–119). Oxfordshire: CAB International.Google Scholar
  9. Case, L. P. (2010). Canine and feline behavior and training: A complete guide to understanding our two best friends. Clifton Park: Delmar, Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
  10. Coppola, C. L., Enns, R. M., & Grandin, T. (2006). Noise in the animal shelter environment: Building design and the effect of daily noise exposure. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 9, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Faragó, T., Pongrácz, P., Range, F., Virányi, Z., & Miklósi, Á. (2010a). The bone is mine’: Affective and referential aspects of dog growls. Animal Behaviour, 79, 917–925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Faragó, T., Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Huber, L., Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2010b). Dogs’ expectation about signalers’ body size by virtue of their growls. PLoS One, 5, e15175, 1–e15175, 8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Feddersen-Petersen, D. U. (2007). Social behaviour of dogs and related canids. In P. Jensen (Ed.), The behavioural biology of dogs (pp. 105–119). Oxfordshire: CAB International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fox, M. W. (1971). Behaviour of wolves, dogs and related canids. London: Jonathan Cape Ltd.Google Scholar
  15. Horowitz, A. (2008). Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Animal Cognition, 12, 107–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Horowitz, A. (2017). Smelling themselves: Dogs investigate their own odours longer when modified in an “olfactory mirror” test. Behavioural Processes, 143, 17–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kaminski, J., & Nitzschner, M. (2013). Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 44, 294–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Landsberg, G., Hunthausen, W., & Ackerman, I. (2003). Handbook of behaviour problems of the dog and cat (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders, Elsevier.Google Scholar
  19. Maros, K., Pongrácz, P., Bárdos, G., Molnár, C., Faragó, T., & Miklósi, Á. (2008). Dogs can discriminate barks from different situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 114, 159–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Miklósi, A. (2007). Dog: Behaviour, evolution and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Mills, D. S., Ramos, D., Estelles, M. G., & Hargrave, G. (2006). A triple blind placebo-controlled investigation into the assessment of the effect of Dog Appeasing Pheromone (DAP) on anxiety related behaviour of problem dogs in the veterinary clinic. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 98, 114–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Pal, S. K. (2003). Urine marking by free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to sex, season, place and posture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 80, 45–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Quaranta, A., Siniscalchi, M., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Current Biology, 17, R199–R201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rooney, N. J. (2016). Welfare concerns about pedigree dog breeding – Almost 10 years of progress? In 22nd FECAVA Eurocongress (pp. 86–90). Vienna: FECAVA, 22–25 June 2016.Google Scholar
  25. Simonet, P., Versteeg, D., & Storie, D. (2005). Dog laughter: Recorded play back reduces stress related aggression in shelter dogs. In N. Clum, S. Silver, & P. Thomas (Eds.), Proceedings of the seventh international conference on environmental enrichment (pp. 170–176). New York: Wildlife Conservation Society.Google Scholar
  26. Stafford, K. (2006). The welfare of dogs. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Taylor, A. M., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2010). Size communication in domestic dog, Canis familiaris, growls. Animal Behaviour, 79, 205–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Udell, M. A. R., Lord, K., Feuerbacher, E. N., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). Dog’s-eye view of canine cognition. In A. Horowitz (Ed.), Domestic dog cognition and behavior (pp. 221–240). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Yeon, S. C. (2007). The vocal communication of canines. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2, 141–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Yin, S., & McCowan, B. (2004). Barking in domestic dog: Context specificity and individual identification. Animal Behaviour, 68, 343–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.ZagrebCroatia

Section editors and affiliations

  • Stephanie Jett
    • 1
  1. 1.Georgia College & State UniversityMobileUSA