Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Means-End Reasoning

  • Anastasia KrasheninnikovaEmail author
Living reference work entry

Latest version View entry history

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1539-2

Synonyms

Definition

Broadly speaking means-end reasoning is concerned with finding means for achieving goals (Pollock 2002). More specifically, it involves the deliberate and planned execution of a chain of actions to achieve a goal and occurs in situations where an obstacle (e.g., a distance between the subject and a desirable item, person) preventing the achievement of the goal must initially be removed (Willatts 1999).

Introduction

In everyday life, we are regularly facing situations which require elaborate sequences of mediating actions to reach a distant goal at the end. For example, imagine a person opening a drawer to take a key to unlock a storeroom to get a ladder needed to reach the door to an out-of-reach vitrine where there is a candy box that can be opened to get some sweets. As this hypothetical problem-solving sequence illustrates (modiefied from Santos et al 2005), the individual steps within a sequence are often separated...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Reference

  1. Alem, S., Perry, C. J., Zhu, X., Loukola, O. J., Ingraham, T., Søvik, E., & Chittka, L. (2016). Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. PLoS Biology, 14(10), 1–28.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Auersperg, A. M. I., Gajdon, G. K., & Huber, L. (2009). Kea (Nestor notabilis) consider spatial relationships between objects in the support problem. Biology Letters, 5(4), 455–458.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0114.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Auersperg, A. M. I., Kacelnik, A., & Von Bayern, A. M. P. (2013). Explorative learning and functional inferences on a five-step means-means-end problem in Goffin’ s cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). PLoS One, 8(7), e68979.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068979.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Bird, C. D., & Emery, N. J. (2009). Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(25), 10370–10375.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901008106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Hanus, D. (2016). Causal reasoning versus associative learning: A useful dichotomy or a strawman battle in comparative psychology? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(3), 241–248.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040235.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Hauser, M. D., Kralik, J., & Botto-Mahan, C. (1999). Problem solving and functional design features: Experiments on cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus oedipus. Animal Behaviour, 57, 565–582.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.1032.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Heinrich, B., & Bugnyar, T. (2005). Testing problem solving in ravens: String-pulling to reach food. Ethology, 111(10), 962–976.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01133.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Herrmann, E., Wobber, V., & Call, J. (2008). Great apes’ (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) understanding of tool functional properties after limited experience. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122(2), 220–230.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.122.2.220.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Irie-Sugimoto, N., Kobayashi, T., Sato, T., & Hasegawa, T. (2008). Evidence of means – End behavior in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Animal Cognition, 11(2), 359–365.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0126-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Jacobs, I. F., & Osvath, M. (2015). The string-pulling paradigm in comparative psychology. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(2), 89–120.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038746.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Köhler, W. (1927). The mentality of aper (2nd ed.). New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
  12. Krasheninnikova, A., Bräger, S., & Wanker, R. (2013). Means-end comprehension in four parrot species: Explained by social complexity. Animal Cognition, 16(5), 755–764.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0609-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Matsuzawa, T. (2001). Primate foundations of human intelligence: A view of tool use in non-human primates and fossil hominids. In T. Matsuzawa (Ed.), Primate origins of human cognition and behavior (pp. 3–25). Tokyo/Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Osthaus, B., Lea, S. E. G., & Slater, A. M. (2005). Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task. Animal Cognition, 8(1), 37–47.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0230-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Piaget, J., & Cook, M. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Pollock, J. L. (2002). The logical foundations of means-end reasoning. In R. Elio (Ed.), Common sense, reasoning, and rationality (pp. 60–77). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for Apes: The Chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. Book reviews.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.orggeochem.2014.10.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Range, F., Möslinger, H., & Virányi, Z. (2012). Domestication has not affected the understanding of means-end connections in dogs. Animal Cognition, 15(4), 597–607.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0488-8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Santos, L. R., Rosati, A., Sproul, C., Spaulding, B., & Hauser, M. D. (2005). Means-means-end tool choice in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Finding the limits on primates’ knowledge of tools. Animal Cognition, 8(4), 236–246.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0246-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Schmidt, G. F., & Cook, R. G. (2006). Mind the gap: Means – End discrimination by pigeons. Animal Behaviour, 71, 599–608.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Shettleworth, S. J. (2012). Do animals have insight, and what is insight anyway? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 66(4), 217–226.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030674.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Taylor, A. H., Medina, F. S., Holzhaider, J. C., Hearne, L. J., Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2010). An investigation into the cognition behind spontaneous string pulling in new caledonian crows. PLoS One, 5(2), e9345.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009345.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Whitt, E., Douglas, M., Osthaus, B., & Hocking, I. (2009). Domestic cats (Felis catus) do not show causal understanding in a string-pulling task. Animal Cognition, 12(5), 739–743.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0228-x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Willatts, P. (1999). Development of means-end behavior in young infants: Pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 651–667.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Wimpenny, J. H., Weir, A. A. S., Clayton, L., Rutz, C., & Kacelnik, A. (2009). Cognitive processes associated with sequential tool use in new Caledonian crows. PLoS One, 4(8), e6471.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006471.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Woodward, J. (2011). A philosopher looks at tool use and causal understanding. In T. McCormack, C. Hoerl, & S. Butterfill (Eds.), Tool use and causal cognition (pp. 1–47). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Behavioural Ecology and Evolutionary GeneticsMax-Planck-Institute for OrnithologySeewiesenGermany
  2. 2.Max-Planck Comparative Cognition Research GroupTenerifeSpain

Section editors and affiliations

  • Dawson Clary
    • 1
  1. 1.University of ManitobaWinnipegCanada