Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders

Living Edition
| Editors: Fred R. Volkmar

Perception

  • Laurent MottronEmail author
  • Isabelle Soulières
  • Michelle Dawson
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6435-8_648-3

Definition

Perception includes the selection, organization, and interpretation of external information coming from the senses up to a complete representation of a stimulus. The extraction of elementary features within primary cortical receiving areas in each sensory modality is called low-level perception. It is domain general such that the same mechanisms are involved in processing social and nonsocial information. At a higher level of integration, there is pattern construction in which elementary features (e.g., of visual or auditory stimuli) are grouped into basic configurations. Visual motion perception depends on information integrated at various levels of the processing hierarchy: first-order motion is perceived in primary areas, while second-order motion requires a network of associative regions. High-level perception involves the matching of constructed configurations with memorized templates as the hierarchy of perceptual processing becomes gradually more domain specific. In...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References and Reading

  1. Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Belmonte, M. K., Cook, E. H., Jr., Anderson, G. M., Rubenstein, J. L., Greenough, W. T., Beckel-Mitchener, A., et al. (2004). Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: Directions for research and targets for therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 9(7), 646–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128(Pt 10), 2430–2441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Caron, M. J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129(Pt 7), 1789–1802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chawarska, K., Volkmar, F., & Klin, A. (2010). Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 178–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dunn, W. (2002). Infant-toddler sensory profile: User’s manual. San Antonio: Therapy skill builders, Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
  7. Frith, U., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Perception in autistic children. In D. J. Cohen, A. Donnellan, & R. Paul (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  8. Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1969). The role of visual and motor cues for normal, subnormal and autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 10, 153–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1998). Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability? Music Perception, 15(3), 291–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1970). Psychological experiments with autistic children. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  11. Jarvinen-Pasley, A., Wallace, G. L., Ramus, F., Happé, F., & Heaton, P. (2008). Enhanced perceptual processing of speech in autism. Developmental Science, 11(1), 109–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kanner, L. (1965). Infantile autism and the schizophrenias. Behavioral Science, 10(4), 412–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1993). A study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain and cognition, 23(2), 279–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Mottron, L., Mineau, S., Martel, G., Bernier, C. S., Berthiaume, C., Dawson, M., et al. (2007). Lateral glances toward moving stimuli among young children with autism: Early regulation of locally oriented perception? Development and Psychopathology, 19(1), 23–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mottron, L., Bouvet, L., Bonnel, A., Samson, F., Burack, J. A., Dawson, M., & Heaton, P. (2013). Veridical mapping in the development of exceptional autistic abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(2), 209–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ornitz, E. M. (1973). Childhood autism. A review of the clinical and experimental literature. California Medicine, 118(4), 21–47.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Ostrolenk, A., Forgeot d’Arc, B., Jelenic, P., Samson, F., & Mottron, L. (2017). Hyperlexia: Systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 79, 134–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Palmer, C. J., Lawson, R. P., & Hohwy, J. (2017). Bayesian approaches to autism: Towards volatility, action, and behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 143(5), 521–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 765–775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Remington, A. M., Swettenham, J. G., & Lavie, N. (2012). Lightening the load: Perceptual load impairs visual detection in typical adults but not in autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 544–551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2005). Annotation: What do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), 1255–1268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rutter, M. (1966). Behavioural and cognitive characteristics of a series of psychotic children. In L. Wing (Ed.), Early childhood autism: Clinical, educational and social aspects (pp. 51–81). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  25. Samson, F., Mottron, L., Soulieres, I., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2012). Enhanced visual functioning in autism: An ALE meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 33(7), 1553–1581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49(22), 2705–2739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., Van der Hallen, R., Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de-Wit, L., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological Review, 121(4), 649–675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Laurent Mottron
    • 1
    Email author
  • Isabelle Soulières
    • 2
  • Michelle Dawson
    • 1
  1. 1.Hôpital Rivière des PrairiesCentre de recherche du CIUSS du Nord de l’île de Montréal et département de psychiatrie de l’Université de MontréalMontréalCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversité du Québec à Montréal (UQAM)MontréalCanada