Skip to main content

Symbolics, Syntactics, and Semantics: Teaching a Language of Maps

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Handbook of the Changing World Language Map
  • 124 Accesses

Abstract

Maps are attempts to communicate, but is mapping a language? Like verbal texts, maps carry several kinds of messages at the same time. Many map symbols represent facts about specific places. At the same time, their positions on the map can reveal distances, directions, patterns, feature associations, and other spatial relationships. Unfortunately, human brains seldom remember shapes and sizes accurately. A workshop “game” helps us understand why – the human visual system processes incoming images through multiple, parallel pathways, only some of which typically lead to conscious awareness. Decades of research by cartographers have given us a number of useful “rules of thumb” about symbol selection, size, visual hierarchy, color sequences, type design and placement, and so forth. More recent research by psychologists and neuroscientists (aided by new brain-scanning technology) has put some of those principles on more solid theoretical grounds. Together, these two lines of research lead to the disturbing conclusion that some popular educational approaches are unlikely to be successful. Educators should approach the teaching of the “vocabulary” and “grammar” of maps by observing what happens in effective foreign-language lessons. Students should be encouraged to practice the basic skills with well-designed maps about topics worth knowing. These activities help students acquire a stock of mental maps of causally important information, which their brains can use to help interpret the spatial patterns, feature associations, analogic positions, and other spatial relationships that they might perceive on new maps. It’s like learning how to learn!

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 849.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 1,099.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Anderson, J. R., Pyke, A. A., & Fincham, J. M. (2016). Hidden stages of cognition revealed in patterns of brain activation. Psychological Science, 27(9), 1215–1226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arguin, M., & Saumier, D. (2000). Conjunction and linear non-separability effects in visual shape encoding. Vision Research, 40, 3099–3115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 280–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blades, M., & Cooke, Z. (1994). Young children’s ability to understand a model as a spatial representation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155(2), 201–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckley, M., & Gaffan, D. (2006). Perirhinal cortical contributions to object perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3), 100–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cate, A. D., & Köhler, S. (2006). The missing whole in perceptual models of perirhinal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(9), 396–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavina-Pratasi, C., Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Milner, A. D. (2010). Separate channels for processing form, texture, and color: Evidence from fMRI adaptation and visual object agnosia. Cerebral Cortex, 20(10), 2819–2832.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh, R., Henik, A., Rubinsten, O., Mohr, H., Dori, H., van de Ven, V., Zorzi, M., Hendler, T., Goebel, R., & Linden, D. E. J. (2005). Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1238–1248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornell, E. H., Sorenson, A., & Mio, T. (2003). Human sense of direction and wayfinding. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 93(2), 399–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vrieze, J. (2017). ‘Science wars’ veteran has a new mission. Science, 358(6360), 159. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.358.6360.159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2003). The neural basis of the Weber-Fechner law: A logarithmic mental number line. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 145–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York: Viking Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., & Rosengran, K. S. (1997). The credible shrinking room: Very young children’s performance with symbolic and non-symbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8(4), 308–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dessalegn, B., & Landau, B. (2008). More than meets the eye; the role of language in binding and maintaining feature conjunctions. Psychological Science, 19, 189–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dow, G. A., & Pick, H. L. (1992). Young children’s use of models and photographs as spatial representations. Cognitive Development, 7(3), 351–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. M., & Liben, L. S. (1991). The development of expertise in geography: A cognitive-developmental approach to geographic education. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 81, 304–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliffe, M. C., Rolls, E. T., & Stringer, S. M. (2002). Invariant recognition of feature combinations in the visual system. Biological Cybernetics, 86, 59–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flannery, J. J. (1971). The relative effectiveness of some common graduated point symbols in the presentation of quantitative data. Cartographica, 8(2), 96–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gattis, M. (Ed.). (2001). Spatial schemas and abstract thought. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gersmehl, P. (1991). Language of maps. NCGE Pathways in Geography #1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gersmehl, P. (2014). Teaching geography (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gersmehl, P., & Gersmehl, C. (2006). Wanted: A concise list of neurologically defensible and assessable spatial-thinking skills. Research in Geographic Education, 8, 5–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gersmehl, P., & Gersmehl, C. (2007). Spatial thinking by young children: What can they do and when can they do it? Journal of Geography, 106(5), 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gersmehl, P., & Gersmehl, C. (2011). Spatial thinking: Where pedagogy meets neuroscience. Problems of Education in the Twenty-First Century, 27, 47–65. (Special issue on geography education).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilmartin, P. P. (1981). Influences of map context on circle perception. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 71(2), 253–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregg, M. (1999). Mapping success: Reversing the Matthew effect. Research in Geographic Education, 1(2), 118–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guelke, L. (1977). Cartographic communication and geographic understanding. Cartographica, 14(1), 129–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, A., Gilaie-Dotan, S., Malach, R., & Bentin, S. (2010). Top-down engagement modulates the neural expression of visual expertise. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2604–2618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, J. B. (1989). Deconstructing the map. Cartographica, 26, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koláčný, A. (1969). Cartographic information—a fundamental concept and term in modern cartography. The Cartographic Journal, 6(1), 47–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1993). Elements of graph design. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (2006). Graph design for the eye and mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, C. H., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Effectiveness of on-screen pinyin in learning Chinese: An expertise reversal for multimedia redundancy effect. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 11–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Yekel, C. A. (1996). Preschoolers’ understanding of plan and oblique maps: The role of geometric and representational correspondence. Child Development, 67, 2780–2796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, J., & Gentner, D. (2005). Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 315–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2007). Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1400–1407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacConnell, A., & Daehler, M. W. (2004). The development of representational insight: Beyond the model/room paradigm. Cognitive Development, 19(3), 345–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacEachren, A. M. (1995). How maps work. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maharawal, M. M., & McElroy, E. (2018). The anti-eviction mapping project: Counter mapping and oral history toward Bay Area housing justice. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 108(2), 380–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mashour, G. A. (2018). The controversial correlates of consciousness. Science, 360(6388), 493–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNaughton, B., Chen, L. L., & Markus, E. J. (1991). “Dead reckoning”, landmark learning, and the sense of direction: A neurophysiological and computational hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 190–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muehrcke, P. (2005). Map use: Reading, analysis, and interpretation. Madison: JP Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (1992). Children’s early ability to solve perspective-taking problems. Developmental Psychology, 28, 635–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nieder, A., & Miller, E. K. (2005). Coding of cognitive magnitude: Compressed scaling of numerical information. Neuron, 37, 149–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfost, M., Hattie, J., Dörfler, T., & Artelt, C. (2014). Individual differences in reading development: A review of 25 years of empirical research on Matthew effects in reading. Review of Educational Research, 84(2), 203–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., & Spencer, J. P. (2007). The emerging spatial mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Prestopnik, J. L., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (2000). The relations among wayfinding strategy use, sense of direction, sex, familiarity, and wayfinding ability. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20, 177–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, M. (2009). Predication and cartographic representation. Synthese, 169(1), 175–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rey, G. D., & Buchwald, F. (2011). The expertise reversal effect: Cognitive load and motivational explanations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(1), 33–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, A. H., & Petchenik, B. B. (1975). The map as a communication system. The Cartographic Journal, 12(1), 7–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossano, M. J., & Morrison, T. T. (1996). Learning from maps: General processes and map-structure influences. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 109–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & PDP Research Group. (1987). Parallel distributed processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shigihara, Y., & Zeki, S. (2014). Parallel processing in the brain’s visual form system: An fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 506–514. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14, 237–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silson, E. H., McKeefry, D. J., Rodgers, J., Gouws, A. D., Hymers, M., & Morland, A. B. (2013). Specialized and independent processing of orientation and shape in visual field maps LO1 and LO2. Nature Neuroscience, 16(3), 267–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soja, E. W. (1989). Postmodern geographies, the reassertion of space on critical social theory. London: Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokal, A., & Bricmont, J. (1998). Fashionable nonsense: Postmodern intellectuals’ abuse of science. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tada, W. L., & Stiles, J. (1996). Developmental change in children’s analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 951–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W., & Stasz, C. (1979). Individual differences in knowledge acquisition from maps. Office of Naval Research R-2375-ONR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., Chun, M. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2008). Neural evidence of statistical learning: Efficient detection of visual regularities without awareness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1934–1945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Energy Information Administration. (2018). Available at https://www.eia.gov/state/maps.php. Accessed 4 July 2018.

  • Uttal, D. H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the development of spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3(3), 247–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., & Elshout, J. J. (1999). Changes in the relation between cognitive and metacognitive skills during the acquisition of expertise. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(4), 509–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Publisher’s note:

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Phil Gersmehl .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Gersmehl, P. (2020). Symbolics, Syntactics, and Semantics: Teaching a Language of Maps. In: Brunn, S., Kehrein, R. (eds) Handbook of the Changing World Language Map. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02438-3_172

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics