Advertisement

Conditioned Drug Effects on Spatial Preference

Critical Evaluation
  • Neal R. Swerdlow
  • David Gilbert
  • George F. Koob
Part of the Neuromethods book series (NM, volume 13)

Abstract

The study of the neural substrates of reinforcement has been facilitated greatly by the diversity of behavioral indices of reinforcement accessible to investigators. Intracranial self-stimlation (ICSS), intravenous self-administration (SA), conditioned approach/avoidance, conditioned taste preference/aversion, and place conditioning (PC) paradigms are distinct tools for studying reinforcement, each with particular advantages that can be manipulated to optimally address questions of interest. In a simple version of the PC paradigm (Fig. 1), animals experience two distinct neutral environments paired spatially and temporally with distinct unconditioned stimuli (UCS). These animals are later given an opportunity to enter either environment, and the time spent in each environment is used as an index of the reinforcing value of each UCS. A reinforcer is operationally defined here as any event that “increases the probability of a response.” Animals spend more time in environments paired with appetitive reinforcers than in those paired with neutral stimuli; conversely, animals spend less time in environments paired with aversive reinforcers than in those paired with neutral stimuli. This choice to spend more or less time in an environment is assumed to be the animals’ expression of their appetitive or aversive experience within that environment; with a positive reinforcing UCS, the previously neutral environmental cues become secondary positive reinforcers, whereas with an aversive UCS, the paired environmental cues become secondary negative reinforcers.
Fig. 1.

Simple version of the place conditioning paradigm. Animals experience two distinct neutral environments (here “black” and “white” shaded) paired spatially and temporally with distinct unconditioned stimuli (here “drug” on days 2, 4, and 6, and “saline” on days 3, 5, and 7). Later (day 8), animals are given an opportunity to enter either environment, and the time spent in each environment is used as an index of the reinforcing value of each UCS. These time values are often compared to a “baseline” preference for each environment (here, measured day 1)

Keywords

Nucleus Accumbens Conditioned Place Preference Spatial Preference Behavioral Measure Neural Substrate 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Advokat C. (1985) Evidence of place conditioning after chronic intrathecal morphine in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 271–277.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Amalric M., Blasco T. A., Smith N. T., Lee D. E., Swerdlow N. R., and Koob G. F. (1986a) “Catatonia” produced by alfentanil is reversed by methylnaloxomum microinjections into the brain. Brain Res 386, 287–295.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Amalric M., Chne E. J, Martinez J. L. Jr., Bloom F. E., and Koob G. F. (1986b) Rewarding properties of beta-endorphin as measured by conditioned place preference. Psychopharmacology 91, 14–19.Google Scholar
  4. Asin K. E. and Wirtshafter D (1985) Clomdine produces a conditioned place preference in rats. Psychopharmacology 85, 383–385.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Asm K. E., Wirtshafter D., and Tabakoff B. (1983) Conditioned place preference and ethanol. Soc Neurosci. Abstr. 9, 1241.Google Scholar
  6. Asin K. E., Wirtshafter D., and Tabakoff B. (1985) Failure to establish a conditioned place reference with ethanol in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 169–173.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Balster R. L. and Schuster C. R. (1973) A comparison of d-amphetamine, amphetamine and methamphetamine self-administration in Resus monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem. Behav 1, 67–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bardo M. T., Miller J. S., and Neisewander J. L. (1984) Conditioned place preference with morphine: The effect of extinction training on the reinforcing CR. Pharmacol Biochem. Behav. 21, 545–549.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Barr G. A., Paredes W., and Bridger W. H. (1985) Place conditioning with morphine and phencyclidine. Dose dependent effects. Life Sci. 36, 363–368.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Beach H. D. (1957) Morphine addiction in rats. Can. J. Psychol. 11, 104–112.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bechara A. and van der Kooy D. (1985) Opposite motivational effects of endogenous opiods in brain and periphery. Nature 314, 533–534.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Best P. J., Best M. R., and Mickley G. A. (1973) Conditioned aversion to distinct environmental stimuli resulting from gastrointestinal distress. J. Comp Physiol. Psychol 85, 250–257PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Blackburn J. R. and Phillips A. G. (1986) Dopamine and preparatory behavior. I. Effects of pimozide, submitted.Google Scholar
  14. Black R. W., Albiniak T., Davis M., and Schumpert J. (1973) A preference in rats for cues associated with intoxication. Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 2, 423–424.Google Scholar
  15. Blander A., Hunt T., Blair R., and Amit Z. (1984) Conditioned place preference An evaluation of morphine’s positive reinforcing properties. Psychopharmacology 84, 124–127.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bozarth M. A. and Wise R. A. (1981) Heroin reward is dependent upon a dopaminergic substrate. Life Sci. 29, 1881–1886.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Byck R. and Van Dyke C. (1977) What Are the Effects of Cocaine in Man?, in NIDA Research Monograph (Peterson R. C. and Stillman R. C., eds.) US Government Printing Office, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  18. Carroll M. E., France C. P., and Meisch R. A. (1981) Intravenous self-administration of etonitazene, cocaine and phencyclidine in rats during food deprivation and satiation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 217, 241–247.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Carr G. D. and White N. M. (1983) Conditioned place preference from intra-accumbens but not intra-caudate amphetamine injections. Life Sci. 33, 2551–2557.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Carr G. D., Phillips A. G., and Fibiger H. C. (1987) Independence of amphetamine reward from locomotor stimulation demonstrated by conditioned place preference, Psychopharmacology, in press.Google Scholar
  21. Cunningham C. L. (1979) Flavor and location aversions produced by ethanol. Behav. Neural Biol. 27, 362–367.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Cunningham C. L. (1980) Spatial aversion conditioning with ethanol. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 14, 263–264.Google Scholar
  23. Dackis C. A and Gold M. S. (1985) New concepts in cocaine addiction: The dopamine depletion hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 9, 469–477.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Davis W. M. and Smith S. G. (1977) Catecholamine mechanisms of reinforcement: Direct assessment by drug self-administration. Life Sci. 20, 483–492.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Deneau G. A., Yanagita T., and Seevers M. H. (1969) Self-administration of psychoactive substances by the monkeys. Psychopharmacolgica 16, 30–48.Google Scholar
  26. De Wit H. and Wise R. A. (1977) Blockade of cocaine reinforcement in rats with the dopamine receptor blocker pimozide, but not with the noradrenergic blockers phentolamine and phenoxybenzamine. Can. J. Psychol. 31, 195–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. di Scala G., Martin-Iverson M. T., Phillips A. G., and Fibiger H. C. (1985) The effects of progabide (SL 76002) on locomotor activity and conditioned place preference induced by d-amphetamine. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 107, 271–274.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Esposito R. and Kornetsky C. (1977) Morphine lowering of self-stimulation thresholds: Lack of tolerance with long-term administration. Science 95, 189–191.Google Scholar
  29. Ettenberg A., Petit H. O., Bloom F. E., and Koob G. F. (1982) Heroin and cocaine self-administration in rats: Mediation by separate neural systems, Psychopharmacology 78, 204–209.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Ettenberg A., van der Kooy D., Le Moal M., Koob G. F., and Bloom F. E. (1983) Can aversive properties of peripherally-injected vasopressin account for its putative role in memory? Behav. Brain Res. 7, 331–350.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Fibiger H. C., Carter D. A., and Phillips A. G. (1976) Decreased intracranial mediation by motor deficits rather than by reduced reward. Psychopharmacology 47, 21–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Fudala P. J., Teoh K. W., and Iwamoto E. T. (1985) Pharmacological characterization of nicotine-induced conditioned place preference. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 237–241.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Garcia J., Kimeldorf D. J., and Hunt E. L. (1958) Spatial avoidance in the rat as a result of exposure to ionizing radiation. Br. J. Radiat. 30, 318–321.Google Scholar
  34. Gilbert D. B. and Cooper S. J. (1983) β-Phenylethylamine-, d-amphetamine-and α-amphetamine-induced place preference conditioning in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 95, 311–314.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Glimcher P. W., Margolin D. H., and Hoebel B. G. (1982) Rewarding effect of neurotensin in the brain. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 400, 422–424.Google Scholar
  36. Glimcher P. W., Margolin D. H., Giovino A. A., and Hoebel B. G. (1984) Neurotensin: A new “reward peptide.”. Brain Res, 291, 119–124.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Glowa J. R. and Barrett J. E. (1983) Drug history modifies the behavioral effects of pentobarbital. Science 220, 333–335.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Goldberg S. R., Spealman R. D., and Goldberg D. M. (1981) Persistant behavior at high rates maintained by intravenous self-administration of nicotine. Science 214, 573–575.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Goldstein J. M. and Malick J. B. (1977) Effects of substance P on medial forebrain bundle self-stimulation in rats following intracerebral administration. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 7, 475–478.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Graeff F. G. and Arisawa E. A. (1978) Effect of intracerebroventricular bradykinin, angiotensin II, and substance P on multiple fixed-interval fixed-ratio responding in rabbits. Psychopharmacology 57, 89–95.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Joyce E. M. and Koob G. F. (1981) Amphetamine-, scopolamine-and caffeine-induced locomotor activity following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology 73, 311–313.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Kalivas P. W., Nemeroff C. B., and Prange A. J. (1981) Increase in spontaneous motor activity following infusion of neurotensin into the ventral tegmental area. Brain Res. 229, 525–529.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Katz R. J. and Gormezano G. (1979) A rapid and inexpensive technique for assessing the reinforcing effects of opiate drugs. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 11, 231–233.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Kelly P. H. and Iversen S. D. (1976) Selective 6-OHDA-induced destruction of mesolimbic dopamine neurons: Abolition of psychostimulant induced locomotor activity in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 40, 45–56.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Kelley A. E. and Stinus L. (1986) Disappearance of hoarding behavior after 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine neurons and its reinstatement with L-dopa. Behav. Neurosa., in press.Google Scholar
  46. Koob G. F., Fray P. J., and Iversen S. D. (1978) Self-stimulation at the lateral hypothalamus and locus coeruleus after specific unilateral lesions of the dopamine system. Brain Res 146, 123–140PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Kumar R. (1972) Morphine dependence in rats: Secondary reinforcement from environmental stimuli. Psychopharmacologia 25, 332–338.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Lett B. T. (1986) Conditioned slowing of stomach emptying produced by Pavlovian pairings of a drug CS or a place with lithium chloride. Psychopharmacology 90, 49–53.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Lyness W. H., Friedle N. M., and Moore K. E. (1979) Destruction of dopaminergic nerve terminals in the nucleus accumbens: Effect on d-amphetamine self-administration. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 11, 553–556.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Mackey W. B. and van der Kooy D. (1985) Neuroleptics block the positive reinforcing effects of amphetamine but not of morphine as measured by place conditioning. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 101–105.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Martin J. C. and Ellinwood E. H. (1974) Conditioned aversions in spatial paradigms following methamphetamine injection. Psychopharmacologia 36, 323–335.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Martin-Iverson M. T., Ortmann R., and Fibiger H. C. (1985) Place preference conditioning with methylphenidate and nomifensine. Brain Res. 332, 59–67.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Martin-Iverson M. T., Radke J. M., and Vincent S. R. (1986) The effects of cysteamine on dopamine-mediated behaviors: Evidence for dopamine-somatostatin interactions in the striatum. Pharmacol Biochem. Behav. 24, 1707–1714.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Messier C. and White N. M. (1984) Contingent and non-contingent actions of sucrose and saccharin reinforcers: Effects on taste preferences and memory. Physiol. Behav. 32, 195–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Mucha R. F. and Herz A. (1986) Preference conditioning produced by opioid active and inactive isomers of levorphanol and morphine in rat. Life Sci. 38, 241–249.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Mucha R. F. and Herz A. (1985) Motivational properties of kappa and mu opioid receptor agonists studied with place and taste preference conditioning. Psychopharmacology 86, 274–280.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Mucha R. F. and Iversen S. D. (1984) Reinforcing properties of morphine and naloxone revealed by conditioned place preference: A procedural examination. Psychopharmacology 82, 241–247.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Mucha R. F., van der Kooy D., O’Shaughnessy M., and Bucenieks P. (1982) Drug reinforcement studied by the use of place conditioning in rat. Brain Res. 243, 91–105.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Mucha R. F., Millan M. J., and Herz A. (1985) Aversive properties of naloxone in non-dependent (naive) rats may involve blockade of central β-endorphin. Psychopharmacology 86, 281–285.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Olds M. E. (1976) Effectiveness of morphine and ineffectiveness of diazepam and phenobarbital on the motivational properties of hypothalamic self-stimulation behavior. Neuropharmacology 15, 117–131.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Olds J. and Milner P. (1954) Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47, 419–427.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Ortmann R. (1985) The conditioned place preference paradigm in rats: Effect of bupropion. Life Sci. 37, 2021–2027.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Pert A. and Sivit C. (1977) Neuroanatomical focus for morphine and endephalin-induced hypermotility. Nature 265, 645.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Petit H. O., Ettenberg A., Bloom F. E., and Koob G. F. (1984) Destruction of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens selectively attentuates cocaine but not heroin self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology 84, 167–173.Google Scholar
  65. Phillips A. G. and LePiane F. G. (1980) Reinforcing effects of morphine microinjection into the ventral tegmental area. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 12, 965–968.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Phillips A. G. and LePiane F. G. (1982) Reward produced by microinjection of (D-ala 2), Met 5-enkephalinamide into the ventral tegmental area. Behav. Brain Res. 5, 225–229.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Phillips A. G., Brook S. M., and Fibiger H. C. (1975) Effects of amphetamine isomers and neuroleptics on self-stimulation from the nucleus accumbens and dorsal noradrenergic bundle. Brain Res. 85, 13–22.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Phillips A. G., Spyraki C., and Fibiger H. C. (1982) Conditioned Place Preference with Amphetamine and Opiates as Reward Stimuli: Attenuation by Haloperidol, in The Neural Basis of Feeding and Reward (Hoebel B. G. and Novin D., eds.) Haer Institute, Brunswick.Google Scholar
  69. Phillips A. G., Broekkamp C. L., and Fibiger H. C. (1983a) Strategies for studying the neurochemical substrates of drug reinforcement in rodents. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiat. 7, 585–590.Google Scholar
  70. Phillips A. G., LePiane F. G., and Fibiger H. C. (1983b) Dopaminergic mediation of reward produced by direct injection of enkephalin into the ventral tegmental area of the rat. Life Sci. 33, 2505–2511.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Pickens R. and Harris W. C. (1968) Self-administration of d-amphetamine by rats. Psychopharmacologia 12, 158–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Pickens R. and Thompson T. (1968) Cocaine-reinforcement behavior in rats: Effects of reinforcement magnitude and fixed ratio size. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 161, 122–129.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Pickens R., Meisch R. A., and Dougherty J. A. (1968) Chemical interactions in amphetamine reinforcement. Psychol. Rep. 23, 1267–1270.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Pickens R., Meisch R. A., and Thompson T. (1978) Drug Self-Administration: An Analysis of the Reinforcing Effects of Drugs, in Handbook of Psychopharmacology vol. 12 (Iversen L. L., Iversen S. D., and Snyder S. H., eds.) Plenum, New York.Google Scholar
  75. Randrupp A. and Munkrad I. (1970) Biochemical, Anatomical and Psychological Investigations of Stereotyped Behaviour Induced by Amphetamiens, in Amphetamines and Related Compounds: Proceedings of the Mario Negri Institute for Pharmacological Research (Costa E. and Garattini S., eds.) Raven, New York.Google Scholar
  76. Reed A., Jr. and Kane A. W. (1972) Phencydlidine (PCP): Another illicit psychedelic drug. J. Psychedelic Drugs 5, 8–12.Google Scholar
  77. Reicher M. A. and Holman E. W. (1977) Location preference and flavor aversion reinforced by amphetamine in rats Anim. Learn. Behav. 5, 343–346.Google Scholar
  78. Reid L. D., Hunter G. A., Beaman C. M., and Hubbell C. L. (1985) Toward understanding ethanol’s capacity to be reinforcing: A conditioned place preference following injections of ethanol. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 483–487.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Resnick R. B., Kestenbaum R. S., Washton A., and Poole D. (1977) Naloxone-precipitated withdrawal: A method for rapid induction onto naltrexone. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 21, 409–413.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Revusky S. H. and Bedarf E. W. (1967) Association of illness with prior ingestion of novel foods. Science 155, 343–346.Google Scholar
  81. Risner M. E. and Goldberg S. R. (1983) A comparison of nicotine and cocaine self-administration in the dog. Fixed-ratio and progressive ratio schedules of intravenous drug infusion. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 224, 319–326.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Risner M. E. and Jones B. E. (1977) Charactristics of β-phenylethylamine self-administration by dog. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 6, 689–696.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Robbins T. W. and Koob G. F. (1980) Selective disruption of displacement behaviour by lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system. Nature 285, 409–412.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Roberts D. C. S. and Koob G. F. (1982) Disruption of cocaine self-administration following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the ventral tegmental area in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 17, 901–904.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Roberts D. C. S., Corcoran M. E., and Fibiger H. C. (1977) On the role of ascending catecholamine systems in intravenous self-administration of cocaine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 6, 615–620.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Roberts D. C. S., Koob G. F., Klonoff P., and Fibiger H. C. (1980) Extinction and recovery of cocaine self-administration following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 12, 781–787.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Rossi N. A. and Reid L. D. (1976) Affective states associated with morphine injections. Physiological Psychol. 4, 269–274.Google Scholar
  88. Schaefer G. J and Michael R. P. (1984) Drug interactions on spontaneous locomotor activity in rats. Neuropharmacology 23, 909–914.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Schenck S., Hunt T., Colle L., and Amit Z. (1982) Isolation versus grouped housing in rats: Differential effects of low doses of heroin in the place preference paradigm. Life Set. 32, 1129–1134.Google Scholar
  90. Schenk S., Hunt T., Colle L., and Amit Z. (1983) Isolation versus grouped housing in rats: Differential effects of low doses of heroin in the place preference paradigm. Life Sci. 32, 1129–11234.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Schenk S., Ellison F., Hunt T., and Amit Z. (1985) An examination of heroin conditioning in preferred and nonpreferred environments and in differentially housed mature and immature rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 215–220.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Schenk S., Hunt T.; Malovechko R., Robertson A., Klukowski G., and Amit Z. (1986) Differential effects of isolation housing on the conditioned place preference produced by cocaine and amphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem Behav 24, 1793–1796.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Schwartz A. S. and Marchok P. L. (1974) Depression of morphine-seeking behavior by dopamine inhibition. Nature 248, 257–258.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Shearman G. T., Hynes M., and Lai H. (1981) Self-Administration of Clonidine by the Rat, in Psychopharmacology of Clonidine (Lai H. and Fielding S., eds.) Liss, New York.Google Scholar
  95. Spyraki C, Fibiger H. C, and Phillips A. G. (1982a) Attenuation by haloperidol of place preference conditioning using food reinforcement. Psychopharmacology 77, 379–382.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Spyraki C, Fibiger H. C., and Phillips A. G. (1982b) Cocaine-induced place preference conditioning: Lack of effects of neuroleptics and 6-hydroxydopamine lesions. Brain Res. 253, 195–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Spyraki C., Fibiger H. C., and Phillips A. G. (1982c) Dopaminergic substrates of amphetamine-induced place preference conditioning. Brain Res. 253, 185–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Spyraki C., Fibiger H. C, and Phillips A. G. (1983) Attenuation of heroin reward in rats by disruption of the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology 79, 278–283.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Spyraki C., Kazandjian A., and Varonos D. (1985) Diazepam-induced place preference conditioning: Appetitive and antiaversive properties. Psychopharmacology 87, 225–232.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Stapleton J. M., Lind M. D., Merriman V. J., Bozarth M. A., and Reid L. D. (1979) Affective consequences and subsequent effects on morphine self-administration of d-ala 2-methionine enkephalin. Physiol. Psychol. 7, 146–152.Google Scholar
  101. Staubli U. and Huston J. P. (1979) Differential effects on learning by ventromedial vs. lateral hypothalamic posttrial injection of substance. P. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav 10, 783–786.Google Scholar
  102. Staubli U. and Huston J. P. (1980) Facilitation of learning by posttrial injection of substance P into the medial septal nucleus. Behav. Brain Res. 1, 245–255PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Staubli U. and Huston J. P. (1985) Central action of substance P: Possible role in reward. Behav. Neural Biol. 43, 100–108.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Staunton D. A., Magistretti P. J., Koob G. F., Shoemaker W. J., and Bloom F. E. (1982) Dopaminergic supersensitivity induced by denervation and chronic receptor blockade is additive. Nature 299, 72–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Stewart R. B. and Grupp L. A. (1981) An investigation of the interaction between and reinforcing properties of food and ethanol using the place preference paradigm. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. 5, 609–613.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Stolerman I. P., Pilcher C. W., and D’Mello G. D. (1978) Stereospecific aversive property of narcotic antagonists in morphine-free rats. Life Set. 22, 1755–1762.Google Scholar
  107. Strecker R. E., Roberts D. C. S., and Koob G. F. (1982) Apomorphine-induced facilitation of ICSS and locomotor behavior following dopamine denervation of the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav 17, 1015–1018PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Swerdlow N. R. and Koob G. F. (1984) Restrained rats learn amphetamine-conditioned locomotion, but not place preference. Psychopharmacology 84, 163–166.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Swerdlow N. R., van der Kooy D., Koob G. F., and Wenger J. R. (1983) Cholecystokinin produces conditioned place-aversions, not place-preferences in food-deprived rats. Evidence against involvement in satiety. Life Sci. 32, 2087–2093.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Swerdlow N. R., Swanson L. W., and Koob G. F. (1984) Electrolytic lesions of the substantia innominata and lateral preoptic region attenuate the’ supersensitive’ locomotor response to apomorphine following denervation of the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 306, 141.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Syme L. A. (1973) Social isolation at weaning. Some effects on two measures of activity. Anim. Learn. Behav. 1, 161–163.Google Scholar
  112. Tombaugh T. N., Grandmaison L. J., and Zito K. A. (1982) Establishment of secondary reinforcement in sign tracking and place preference tests following pimozide treatment. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 17, 665–670.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. van der Kooy D., Mucha R. F., O’Shaughnessy M., and Bucenieks P. (1979) Reinforcing effects of opiate drugs. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 11, 231–233.Google Scholar
  114. van der Kooy D., O’Shaughnessy M., Mucha R. F., and Kalant H. (1983a) Motivational properties of ethanol in naive rats as studied by place conditioning. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 19, 441–445.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. van der Kooy D., Swerdlow N. R., and Koob G. F. (1983b) Paradoxical reinforcing properties of apomorphine: Effects of nucleus accumbens and area postrema lesions. Brain Res. 259, 111–118.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Van Ree J. M., Gaffori C, and deWied D. (1983) In rats the behavioral profile of CCK-8 related peptides resembles that of antipsychotic agents. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 93, 63–78.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. White N. M. and Carr G. D. (1985) The conditioned place preference is affected by two independent reinforcement processes. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 23, 37–42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Wise R. A. (1978) Catecholamine theories of reward: A critical review. Brain Res. 152, 215–217.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. Wise R. A. (1982) Neuroleptics and operant behaior: The anhedonia hypothesis. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 39–87.Google Scholar
  120. Woolverton W. L., Wessinger W. D., and Balster R. L. (1982) Reinforcing properties of clonidine in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology 77, 17–23.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  121. Yokel R. A. and Pickens R. (1974) Drug level of d-and l-amphetamine during intravenous self-administration. Psychopharmacologia 34, 233–264.Google Scholar
  122. Young P. T. and Masden C. H. (1963) Individual isohedons in sucrose-sodium chloride and sucrose-saccharin gustatory areas. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 56, 903–909.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  123. Zito K. A., Vickers G., and Roberts D. C. S. (1985) Disruption of cocaine and heroin self-administration following kainic acid lesions of the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 23, 1029–1036.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Humana Press Inc. 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Neal R. Swerdlow
    • 1
  • David Gilbert
    • 2
  • George F. Koob
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of CaliforniaSan Diego, La Jolla
  2. 2.Department of PharmacologyUniversity of CaliforniaIrvine
  3. 3.Division of Preclinical Neuroscience and EndocrinologyScripps Clinic and Research FoundationLa Jolla

Personalised recommendations