Skip to main content
Log in

“Physical Space First!”: A Corpus-Based Study on the Use of Localizer ‘Shang’ in Early Child Mandarin

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Corpus Pragmatics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study investigated the pragmatic use of localizer ‘shang (上)’ by 168 Mandarin-speaking preschoolers (aged 2;6, 3;6, 4;6, 5;6) in the Early Childhood Mandarin Corpus (ECMC) (Li and Tse in Early Childhood Mandarin Corpus, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, 2011). Six types of pragmatic meaning of ‘shang (上)’ were proposed based on literature review, the four about physical space (including the meaning of supporting, attaching, positioning and containing) were produced by 62 children with 118 tokens from the four age groups, whereas the two about psychological space were not found in the corpus. No significant age and gender differences were found in the production of ‘Noun (N) + shang (上)’ expression. The localizers, ‘shang (上, on)’ and ‘li (里, in)’ were often used interchangeably, although they have different meanings in spatial expression. The developmental trends of use of ‘shang (上)’ were discussed with reference to cultural context, cognitive development and language acquisition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bowerman, M. (1996). Learning how to structure space for language: A cross-linguistic perspective. In P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 385–436). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. (2001). Shaping meaning for language: Universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475–511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. (2003). Space under construction: Language-specific spatial categorization in first language acquisition. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadon (Eds.), Language in mind: Advance in the study of language and thought (pp. 387–427). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P., George, J., & Inch, R. (1990). Sex differences and the role of figurai complexity in determining the rate of mental rotation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 467–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casasola, M., & Cohen, L. B. (2002). Infant categorization of containment, support, and tight-fit spatial relationships. Developmental Science, 5, 247–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chu, Z. (1997). Xian dai Han you fang suo xi tong yan jiu = Xìandài Hànyǔ fāngsuǒ xìtǒng yánjiū. Wuchang: Hua zhong shi fan da xue chu ban she.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1973a). Nonlinguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings. Cognition, 2, 161–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973b). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 329–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fang, J. M. (2002). Properties and types of the spatial region categories in Chinese. Chinese Teaching in the World, 3, 37–48 [In Chinese: 方经民. 论汉语空间区域范畴的性质和类型世界汉语教学, 2002, (3): 37–48].

  • Ge, T. (2004). Cognitive analyses on ‘X上’ and ‘X里’. Journal of College of Chinese in Jinan University, 1, 59–68 [In Chinese: 葛婷. “X 上” 和 “X 里” 的认知分析. 暨南大学华文学院学报, (1), 59–68].

  • Gou, R. L. (2004). The cognitive basis of Chinese local words “Shang”“Xia” and teaching Chinese language as a second language. Applied Linguistics, 4, 69–75 [In Chinese: 缑瑞隆. 方位词 “上”“下” 的语义认知基础与对外汉语教学. 语言文字应用, (4), 69–75].

  • Griffiths, P. (2006). Introduction to English semantics and pragmatics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jia, H. X. (2010). The semantic development of the spatial prepositions by a Putonghua-speaking child: A case study. Applied Linguistics, 4, 514–525 [In Chinese: 贾红霞. 普通话儿童方位词发展的个案研究 [J]. 语言文字应用, 2010, (4): 514–525].

  • Johnston, J., & Slobin, D. I. (1979). The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish. Journal of Child Language, 6, 529–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, B. F. (2002). “Well you can’t put your swimsuit on top of your pants!”: Child-mother uses of in and on. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 31st Stanford child language research forum (pp. 10–21). Standford: CSLI.

  • Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 16–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kong, L. D., & Wang, X. R. (2002). Related issues of the acquisition of locative postpositions of young children. Chinese Language, 2, 111–117 [In Chinese: 孔令达, & 王祥荣.儿童语言中方位词的习得及相关问题 [J]. 中国语文, 2002, (2): 111–117].

  • Kravchenko, A. V. (2001). Russian verbs of spatial orientation STAND, SIT, LIE. In E. Nemeth (Ed.), Cognition in language use: selected papers from the 7th international pragmatics conference (Vol. 1, pp. 216–225). Derived from: http://cogprints.org/4003/1/StandSitLie.pdf.

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicage press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., & Tse, S. K. (2011). Early Childhood Mandarin Corpus. Pokfulam: The University of Hong Kong.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, N. S. (1994). Express spatial relations among objects in Chinese. Chinese Language, 3, 169–179 [In Chinese: 刘宁生. 汉语怎样表达物体的空间关系 [J]. 中国语文, 1994, (3):169–179].

  • Lv, S. X. (1992). Xian Dai Han Yu Ba Bai Ci. Jiangsu: The Commercial Press [In Chinese: 吕叔湘. 现代汉语八百词 [M]. 江苏: 商务印书馆, 1992].

  • Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Modern Chinese Dictionary. (1999). ‘shang (上)’. P1105. The Commercial Press [In Chinese: 现代汉语词典. 1999. 商务印书馆].

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tong, S. Q. (2006). The cognitive basis of “Shang” as a noun of locality. Applied Linguistics, 1, 87–92 [In Chinese: 童盛强. 也说方位词 “上” 的语义认知基础-兼与缑瑞隆先生商榷. 语言文字应用, 2006(1), 87–92].

  • Tse, S. K., Chan, C., Li, H., & Kwong, S. M. (2002). Sex differences in syntactic development: Evidence from Cantonese-speaking preschoolers in Hong Kong. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(6), 509–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tse, S. K., & Li, H. (2011). Early child Cantonese: facts and implications (Studies on Language Acquisition [SOLA] Series, 42). Berlin: Gruyter Mouton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vasta, R., Knott, J. Α., & Gaze, C. E. (1996). Can spatial training erase the gender differences on the water-level task? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 549–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, H. P., & Wei, X. (2007). Semantics of spatial expressions in and on in English and Li and Shang in Chinese: A cognitive approach. The Journal of Specialized Translation, 30 [In Chinese: 武和平, & 魏行. 英汉空间方所表达的认知语义分析. 解放军外国语学院学报, 30(3)].

  • Xiong, S. L. (2014). A study of locatives acquisition on Mandarin child. Unpublished Master’s Degree Thesis of Capital Normal University [In Chinese: 熊丽双.普通话早期儿童方位词习得个案研究 [D] .首都师范大学: 硕士学位论文, 2014].

  • Yip, P. C., & Rimmington, D. (2006). Chinese: An essential grammar. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, K. D. (2008). Spatial relations and the related interpretation of cognitive linguistic explanations. Journal of Henan University (Social Science), 48(1), 1–8 [In Chinese: 张克定. (2008). 空间关系及其语言表达的认知语言学阐释. 河南大学学报 (社会科学版), 48(1), 1–8].

Download references

Acknowledgements

This article is based on a project (RGC Ref No. 747109) funded to Dr. Hui Li by the Research Grants Council of the Government of the Hong Kong SAR. Thanks are given to all the participating children and their parents.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hui Li.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wu, D., Lau, C. & Li, H. “Physical Space First!”: A Corpus-Based Study on the Use of Localizer ‘Shang’ in Early Child Mandarin. Corpus Pragmatics 2, 149–166 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41701-017-0027-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41701-017-0027-6

Keywords

Navigation