Skip to main content
Log in

Dugnad: A Fact and a Narrative of Norwegian Prosocial Behavior

  • Cultural and Behavioral Systems Science
  • Published:
Perspectives on Behavior Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Evolved mechanisms of phenotypic plasticity, which are evolutionary processes in their own right, enable species to respond adaptively to their environments. The Scandinavian countries, and Norway in particular, have for many years scored exceptionally high on lists of life quality, economic indicators, and measures of happiness. We propose that learning prosocial and cooperative behavior, which is central in a particular Norwegian cultural practice, dugnad, plays a role in the country’s success story. Dugnad is a Norwegian term for a type of voluntary work carried out as a community or collective and traditionally involving a social gathering. Dugnad has a long history in Norway, and it is a well-established cultural practice that has led to and still maintains significant social benefits. Dugnad is arranged in virtually all communities such as kindergartens, neighborhoods, schools, and organizations. Participation in dugnad gatherings is generally expected. Children from a young age are involved in dugnad. Dugnad activities are based on cooperation and can include anything from arranging a spring cleaning in the local community to building a club house for your children’s sports club. This paper discusses dugnad as a cultural practice that creates an environment that nurtures prosocial and cooperative activities. From a behavior analytic, selectionist perspective, we propose a non-domain-specific learning mechanism for dugnad-typical prosocial and cooperative behavior analogous to the phylogenetic evolutionary mechanism of group selection. Contingencies can lead to and maintain dugnad activities when extended behavioral patterns are selected as wholes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Online source without page numbers that we edited for grammar.

  2. In the following, we omit italics when referring to the cultural practice of dugnad, instead of the term dugnad.

  3. We omit “a constellation of values [and] attitudes” (Biglan, 2015, p. 16), which are part of both of Biglan’s definitions, but which we regard to be inseparable from behavior.

  4. Further vocabulary that Glenn et al. (2016) specifically developed for describing cultural selection processes includes metacontingency, macrobehavior, macrocontingency, culturo-behavioral lineage, culturant, and cultural cusp. Our analyses are compatible with processes that might describe such a cultural level of selection (see Krispin, 2016, 2017, for an application of the metacontingency vocabulary). However, a conceptualization of dugnad as a result of cultural level selection in terms of metacontingencies would here distract from our goal to outline the contribution of natural and operant selection to the cultural practice of dugnad. An analysis in terms of these cultural selection concepts seems inept for our present purposes of 1) discussing the contribution of operant selection of less-extended acts to the maintenance of the cultural practice of dugnad and 2) suggesting that, in dugnad participation, both selection of temporally extended behavior of individuals and that produced by several group members together is likewise selected by PIEs.

  5. We regard so-called altruistic behavior and prosocial or cooperative behavior as gradually different in their cost-benefit distribution and in the temporal distance between the cost and the benefit, but not as different in kind. Prosocial and altruistic behavior would be categorically different only if altruistic behavior were defined as nonreinforced behavior—a definition that would defy behavior analysis (Rachlin & Locey, 2011).

  6. This is not to claim that inheritance of altruistic tendencies is impossible or to deny that babies are more likely to reinforce the behavior of a person they have observed to cooperate (which Biglan, 2015, uses as evidence of “wired-in tendencies” [p. 16]). However, here we would like to spread hope by outlining how prosocial behavior can be learned just as we can learn self-control (Locey, Jones, & Rachlin, 2013).

  7. Note that paying your share for the skip is independent of your actual dugnad participation. Carrying the washing machine is part of participation, but both activities are independent of whether it was you who “disposed” of the washing mashing by placing it in front of the exit in the first place.

References

  • Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22(1), 231–242. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1974.22-231.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1995). Rules, culture, and fitness. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 1–21.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2000). Being concrete about culture and cultural evolution. In F. Tonneau & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology: Evolution, culture, and behavior (pp. 181–212). Boston, MA: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2012). Rethinking reinforcement: Allocation, induction, and contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 97(1), 101–124. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2012.97-101.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2013). What counts as behavior? The molar multiscale view. The Behavior Analyst, 36(2), 283–293.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2016). Driven by consequences: The multiscale molar view of choice. Managerial & Decision Economics, 37(4–5), 239–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2018). Multiscale behavior analysis and molar behaviorism: An overview. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110(3), 302–322. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beier, K. (2011). Beyond the dichotomy of individualism and solidarity: Participation in biobank research in Sweden and Norway. In C. Lenk, N. Hoppe, K. Beier, & C. Wiesemann (Eds.), Human tissue research: A discussion of the ethical and legal challenges from a European perspective (pp. 65–75). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, H., & Trägårdh, L. (2011). Social trust and radical individualism: The paradox at the heart of Nordic capitalism. In Paper presented at the World Economic Forum, "Shared Norms for the New Reality: The Nordic Way,". Davos, Stockholm: Sweden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biglan, A. (2015). The nurture effect: How the science of human behavior can improve our lives and our world. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biglan, A., Flay, B. R., Embry, D. D., & Sandler, I. N. (2012). The critical role of nurturing environments for promoting human well-being. American Psychologist, 67(4), 257–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biglan, A., & Glenn, S. S. (2013). Toward prosocial behavior and environments: Behavioral and cultural contingencies in a public health framework. In G. J. Madden, W. V. Dube, T. D. Hackenberg, G. P. Hanley, & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), APA handbook of behavior analysis, Vol. 2: Translating principles into practice. (pp. 255–275). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Borba, A., Da Silva, B. R., dos Anjos Cabral, P. A., de Souza, L. B., Leite, F. L., Tourinho, E. Z. J. B., & Issues, S. (2014a). Effects of exposure to macrocontingencies in isolation and social situations in the production of ethical self-control. 23, 5–19.

  • Borba, A., Tourinho, E. Z., & Glenn, S. S. (2014b). Establishing the macrobehavior of ethical self-control in an arrangement of macrocontingencies in two microcultures. Behavior & Social Issues, 23, 68–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borba, A., Tourinho, E. Z., & Glenn, S. S. (2017). Effects of cultural consequences on the interlocking behavioral contingencies of ethical self-control. The Psychological Record, 67(3), 399–411. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0231-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Gintis, H., Bowles, S., & Richerson, P. (2003). The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(6), 3531–3535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J., & Rachlin, H. (1999). Self-control and social cooperation. Behavioural Processes, 47(2), 65–72.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, N. F. (2006). The Nordic model of welfare: A historical reappraisal. Copenhagen, Denmark: Museum Tusculanum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Couto, K. C., & Sandaker, I. (2016). Natural, behavioral and cultural selection analysis: An integrative approach. Behavior & Social Issues, 25, 54–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype: The gene as the unit of selection. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (2012). The world until yesterday: What can we learn from traditional societies? London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dølvik, J. E. (2013). Grunnpilarene i de nordiske modellene: et tilbakeblikk på arbeidslivs-og velferdsregimenes utvikling. [The pillars of the Nordic models: A look back at the development of the working life and welfare regimes]. Oslo, Norway: Fafo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edlund, J. (1999). Trust in government and welfare regimes: Attitudes to redistribution and financial cheating in the USA and Norway. European Journal of Political Research, 35(3), 341–370. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.00452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eklund, K. (2011). Nordic capitalism: Lessons learned. Paper presented at the World Economic Forum, "The Nordic Way: Shared Norms for the New Reality, Davos, Stockholm Sweden.

  • European-Central-Bank. (2015). Payment statistics for 2014 [press release].

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785–791.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1904). Eugenics: Its definition, scope, and aims. American Journal of Sociology, 10(1), 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S. (1988). Contingencies and metacontingencies: Toward a synthesis of behavior analysis and cultural materialism. The Behavior Analyst, 11(2), 161–179 Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22478011.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S. (2003). Operant contingencies and the origin of cultures. In K. A. Lattal & P. Chase (Eds.), Behavior theory and philosophy (pp. 223–242). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S. (2004). Individual behavior, culture, and social change. The Behavior Analyst, 27(2), 133–151. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03393175.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, S. S., Malott, M. E., Andery, M. A. P. A., Benvenuti, M., Houmanfar, R. A., Sandaker, I., et al. (2016). Toward consistent terminology in a behaviorist approach to cultural analysis. Behavior & Social Issues, 25, 11–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greve, B. (2007). What characterise the Nordic welfare state model. Journal of Social Sciences, 3(2), 43–51. https://doi.org/10.3844/jssp.2007.43.51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grunow, A., & Neuringer, A. (2002). Learning to vary and varying to learn. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 250–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162, 1243–1248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haugestad, A. K. (2003). The dugnad: Sustainable development and sustainable consumption in Norway. Paper presented at the meeting of Environmental Social Sciences (NESS), Turku/Åbo, Finland.

  • Helliwell, J., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (2017). World happiness report 2017. New York, NY: Sustainable Development Solutions Network Retrieved from http://worldhappiness.report/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13(2), 243–266.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hilson, M. (2008). The Nordic model: Scandinavia since 1945. London: Reaktion Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Institute for Social Research. (2008). The Norwegian tradition of the ”Dugnad”. Retrieved from https://www.samfunnsforskning.no/english/projects/the-norwegian-tradition-of-the-dugnad%2D%2Deng.html.

  • Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2009). Delay, probability, and social discounting in a public goods game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91(1), 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.91-61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kagge, G. (2019). Slik stakk politikerne av med nasjonalordet "dugnad." [This is how politicians nicked the word "dugnad"]. Aftenposten. Retrieved from https://www.aftenposten.no/norge/i/dOqVgX/Slik-stakk-politikerne-av-med-nasjonalordet-dugnad.

  • Kildal, C., & Bjerke, L. (2018). The Nordic puzzle: Why are the Nordic countries ranked as the world’s best countries to live in? And can their good fortune last? Retrieved from https://www.sum.uio.no/english/research/news-and-events/news/2018/the-nordic-puzzle.html.

  • Klepp, A. (2001). From neighbourly duty to national rhetoric: An analysis of the shifting meanings of Norwegian dugnad. Ethnologia Scandinavica, 31, 82–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knutsen, O. (2017). The Nordic models in political science: Challenged, but still viable? Oslo, Norway: Fagbokforlaget.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraglund, K. O., & Enjolras, B. (2017). Norsk frivillighet: Utviklingstrender og samfunnseffekter. Avslutningsrapport for Senter for forskning på sivilsamfunn og frivillig sektor [Norwegian volunteerism: Development trends and social effects. Closing report of the Center for Research on Civil Society and the Voluntary Sector.]. Rapport fra Senter for forskning på sivilsamfunn og frivillig sektor.

  • Krasnow, M. M., & Delton, A. W. (2016). The sketch is blank: no evidence for an explanatory role for cultural group selection. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 39, e58. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krasnow, M. M., Delton, A. W., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2015). Group cooperation without group selection: Modest punishment can recruit much cooperation. PloS one, 10(4), e0124561.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krispin, J. (2016). What is the metacontingency? Deconstructing claims of emergence and cultural-level selection. Behavior & Social Issues, 25, 28–41. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v25i0.6186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krispin, J. (2017). Positive feedback loops of metacontingencies: A new conceptualization of cultural-level selection. Behavior & Social Issues, 26, 95–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kvist, J. (2012). Changing social equality: The Nordic welfare model in the 21st century. Bristol, UK: Policy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenk, C. (2011). Human tissue research: A European perspective on the ethical and legal challenges. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindholm, M. (2012). Evolusjon: Naturens kulturhistorie. Spartacus Forlag.

  • Locey, M. L., Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2013). Self-control and altruism. In G. J. Madden, W. V. Dube, T. D. Hackenberg, G. P. Hanley, & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), APA handbook of behavior analysis, Vol. 1: Methods and principles (pp. 463–481). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locey, M. L., & Rachlin, H. (2015). Altruism and anonymity: A behavioral analysis. Behavioural Processes, 118, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lorentzen, H., & Dugstad, L. (2011). Den norske dugnaden: historie, kultur og fellesskap. [The Norwegian "Dugnad": History, culture, and community ]. Høyskoleforlaget, Norway: Norwegian Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Midttun, A., Witoszek, N., Joly, C., Karlsson-Vinkhuyzen, S., Olsen, P. I., Olsson, L., . . . Østergård, U. (2011). The Nordic model: Is it sustainable and exportable? CERES Working Papers, no. 2020110309. Retrieved from http://www.ceres21.org/media/UserMedia/Nordic%20model_original%2020110309.pdf.

  • Mobekk, H., Karevold, K. I., Tran, H., & Stjernen, K. (2018). Fish versus meat: Nudging the healthier choice off food in hotel lunch buffets. Norsk Tidsskrift for Ernæring, 16(3), 6–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (2004). Reinforced variability in animals and people: Implications for adaptive action. American Psychologist, 59(9), 891–906.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-control: Beyond commitment. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 18(1), 109–121. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00037602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (2004). The science of self-control. Cambrige, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (2019). Group selection in behavioral evolution. Behavioural Processes, 161, 65–72.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., & Green, L. (1972). Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17(1), 15–22.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., & Locey, M. (2011). A behavioral analysis of altruism. Behavioural Processes, 87(1), 25–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 66(1), 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It's not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 43(3), 151–160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P., Baldini, R., Bell, A., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., et al. (2015). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 39(E43), 1–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sejersted, F. (2010). Nasjonal dugnad. [National "Dugnad"]. Morgenbladet.

  • Sennett, R. (2012). Together: The rituals, pleasures and politics of cooperation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, C. (2016). Cultural group selection in the light of the selection of extended behavioral patterns. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 39, 39–40. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1400106X,e30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, C. (2017). Why Norwegians don’t have their pigs in the forest: Illuminating Nordic "co-operation.". Behavior & Social Issues, 26, 172–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, C., & Hessen, D. O. (2019). Selection as a domain-general evolutionary process. Behavioural Processes, 161, 3–16 Retrieved from http://www.journals.elsevier.com/behavioural-processes.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden two. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1972). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213(4507), 501–504. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7244649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soltis, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1995). Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection? An empirical test. Current Anthropology, 36(3), 473–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teitelbaum, P. (1977). Levels of integration of the operant. In J. E. R. Staddon & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 7–27). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P. (2007). War and peace and war: The rise and fall of empires. New York, NY: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Na1tions. (2015). Human development report. New York, NY: United Nations Development Programme.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415–432.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S. (1975). A theory of group selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 72(1), 143–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S. (2015). Does altruism exist?: Culture, genes, and the welfare of others. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., Hayes, S., Biglan, A., & Embry, D. (2014). Evolving the future: Toward a science of intentional change. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 37(4), 395–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Hessen, D. (2018). Cooperation, competition and multi- level selection: A new paradigm for understanding the Nordic model. In N. Witoszek & A. Midttun (Eds.), Sustainable modernity: The Nordic model and beyond (pp. 30–47). London, UK: Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Hessen, D. O. (2014). Blueprint for the global village. Cliodynamics: The Journal of Quantitative History & Cultural Evolution, 5(1), 123–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Kniffin, K. M. (1999). Multilevel selection and the social transmission of behavior. Human Nature, 10(3), 291–310.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1998). Multilevel selection and the return of group-level functionalism. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 21(2), 305–306. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x98221194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witoszek, N., & Midttun, A. (2018). Sustainable modernity: The Nordic Model and beyond. London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H. (1968). The serial organization of sucking in the young infant. Pediatrics, 42(6), 943–956.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank David Sloan Wilson, Anthony Biglan, Ingunn Sandaker, Nina Witoszek, Kalliu Carvalho Couto, and Asle Fagerstrøm for their review and feedback on earlier drafts of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carsta Simon.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Simon, C., Mobekk, H. Dugnad: A Fact and a Narrative of Norwegian Prosocial Behavior. Perspect Behav Sci 42, 815–834 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-019-00227-w

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-019-00227-w

Keywords

Navigation