Skip to main content

Text Processing Within Classroom Contexts

  • Chapter
Missing the Meaning
  • 51 Accesses

Abstract

It is now almost thirty years since there was a radical shift within the research literature on text comprehension. Until the 1970s, with few exceptions, the psychological literature was dominated by a “bottom-up” model of reading. The reader moved from letter—sound decoding through word recognition and sentence processing to the acquisition of meaning from the text. Once a text was decoded comprehension was assumed to be relatively automatic. This view of reading was consistent with the behaviorist orientation that had dominated psychology in the preceding decades and had moved psychologists to study not text comprehension but its assumed component processes and to do this within laboratory settings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 19.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, R.C., Reynolds, R.R., Schallert, D.L., & Goetz, E.T. (1977). Frameworks for comprehending discourse. American Educational Research Journal, 14 (4), 367–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R.C. & Davison, A. (1988). Conceptual and empirical bases of readability formulas. In A. Davison & G.M. Green (Eds.), Linguistic complexity and text comprehension: Readability issues reconsidered (pp. 22–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bazerman, C. (1988). Shaping written knowledge: The genre and activity of the experimental article in science. Madison: University of Wiscons in Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L. (1993). On reading: A survey of recent research and proposals for the future. In A.P. Sweet & J.I. Anderson (Eds.), Reading research into the year 2000 (pp. 65–87). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I.L. & McKeown, M. G. (1989). Expository text for young readers: The issue of coherence. In L. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 47–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloome, D. (Ed.) (1989) Classrooms and literacy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D. & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J.D. & Johnson, M.K. (1973). Consideration of problems of comprehension. In W.G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrell, P. (1983). Three components of background knowledge in reading comprehension. Language Learning, 33, 183–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Q. & Donin, J. (1997). Discourse processing of first- and second-language biology texts: Effects of domain-specific knowledge and language proficiency. Modern Language Journal, 81 (2), 209–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., T.H. Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., Bassok, M. (1989). Learning from examples via self-explanations. In L. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 251–282). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C.A. & Brewer, W.F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. & Engestrom, Y. (1995). Commentary. Human Development, 38 (1), 19–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connor, U. (1996). Contrastive rhetoric: Cross-cultural aspects of second-language writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Donin, J., Doehring, D. G., & Browns, E (1991). Text comprehension and reading achievement in orally-educated hearing-impaired children. Discourse Processes, 14, 307–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donin, J. & Silva, M. (1993). The relationship between first- and second-language reading comprehension of occupation-specific texts. Language Learning, 43 (3), 377–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. & Goodwin, C. (Eds.). (1992). Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, C. H. (1975). Representing logical and semantic structure of knowledge acquired from discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 371–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, C.H. (1986). Cognitive models and discourse analysis. In C.R. Cooper & S. Greenbaum (Eds.), Studying writing: Linguistic approaches (pp. 227–267). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, C.H. (1989). Text comprehension in functional task domains. In D. Bloome (Ed.), Classrooms and literacy (pp. 189–222). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, C.H. & Donin, J. (1991). Constructing multiple semantic representations in comprehending and producing discourse. In G. Denhière & J.P. Rossi (Eds.), Texts and text processing (pp. 19–44). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, C.H. & Donin, J. (August 1996). Conceptual processing in text comprehension and learning through discourse. Paper presented at invited symposium at XXVI International Congress of Psychology, Montréal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goyette, E. & Donin, J. (April 1993). Second-language text comprehension: Knowledge and text types. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabe, W. & Kaplan, R. B. (1996). Theory and practice of writing. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1997). On claims that answer the wrong questions. Educational Researcher, 26, 5–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guzzetti, B.J., Snyder, T.E., Glass, G.V., & Gamas, W.S. (1993). Promoting conceptual change in science: A comparative meta-analysis of instructional interventions from reading education and science education. Reading Research Quarterly, 28 (2), 116–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M.A.K. & Martin, J.R. (1993). Writing science: Literacy and discursive power. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinds, J. (1990). Inductive, deductive, quasi-inductive: Expository writing in Japanese, Korean, and Thai. In U. Connor & A.M. Johns (Eds.), Coherence in writing: Research and pedagogical perspectives. Alexandria, VA: TESOL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hymes, D. (1972). On communicative competence. In J.B. Pride & J. Holmes (Eds.), Sociolinguistics (pp. 269–293). Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, P. (1981). Effects on reading comprehension of language complexity and cultural background of a text. TESOL Quarterly, 15, 169–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G.J. & Green, J. (1998). The social nature of knowing. In B. Guzzetti & C. Hynd (Eds.), Perspectives on conceptual change: Toward a sociocultural perspective on conceptual change and knowledge construction (pp. 145–182). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. & Greene, E. (1978). The role of culture-specific schemata in the comprehension and recall of stories. Discourse Processes, 1, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. & Keenan, J. M. (1973). Reading rate and retention as a function of the number of propositions in the base structure of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 257–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. & Vipond, D. (1979). Reading comprehension and readabillity in educational practice and psychological theory. In L.G. Nilson (Ed.), Perspectives on memory research (pp. 325–365). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, G. (1989). Linguistic processes in sociocultural practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J.A. (1986). Children reading and writing: Structures and strategies. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longacre, R.E. (1996). The grammar of discourse. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, N. & Glock, M.D. (1978). Comprehension of connected discourse: A study of the relationship between the structure of text and information recalled. Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 10–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B.J.F. & Freedle, R. (1984). Effects of discourse type on recall. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 121–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1990). The cooperative language processors: Semantic influences in an autonomous syntax. In D.A. Balota, G.B. Flores d’Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 205–230). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Foorman, B., Perfetti, C., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. (2002). How should reading be taught? Scientific American, 286 (3), 85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothkopf, E.Z. & Koether, M.E. (1978). Instructional effects of discrepancies in content and organization between study goals and information sources. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70 (1), 67–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D.G. Bobrow and A. Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding (pp.211–236). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E. (1980). Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In Spiro, R., Bruce, B.C., & Brewer, W.F. (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 33–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanford, A.J. & Garrod, S.C. (1998). The role of scenario mapping in text comprehension. Discourse Process, 26 (2), 159–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffenson, M. S., Joag-Dev, C., & Anderson, R. C. (1979). A cross-cultural perspective on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 15, 10–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sticht, T., Beck, L.B., Hauke, R.N., Kleiman, G.M., & James, J.H. (1974). Auding and reading. Alexandria, VA: Human Resources Research Organization.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stubbs, M. (1996). Text and corpus analysis. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swales, J. W. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, L. (2001). Evaluating the language of explanation in different types of explanations in junior high school science texts. International Journal of Science Education, 23 (6), 585–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, T.A. (1997). Discourse as structure and process. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2004 Alan Peacock and Ailie Cleghorn

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Donin, J. (2004). Text Processing Within Classroom Contexts. In: Peacock, A., Cleghorn, A. (eds) Missing the Meaning. Palgrave Macmillan, New York. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781403982285_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics