Skip to main content

Abstract

In a classic Zen story, retold by Kapleau, a man approached Ikkyu, a Zen master, and asked for the highest wisdom.

To follow the lead of Nietzsche, James, and Jung, if the higher spiritual traditions of humanity might actually refer to something important... then some sort of account of how they could occur as an expression of the structure of the human mind will be necessary. —H.T. Hunt, On the Nature of Consciousness

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abdullaev, Y.G. and Posner, M.I. 1997. Time course of activating brain areas in generating verbal associations. Psychological Science, 8, 56–59. Aitken, R. 1982. Taking the Path of Zen. NewYork: North Point Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. 1988. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. — 1997. In the Theater of Consciousness:TheWorkspace of the Mind. NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley,A. 1987. Human Memory:Theory and Practice. London: Psychology Press. Bargh, J. and Chartrand, T. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore, S. 2004. Consciousness:An Introduction. NewYork: Oxford University Press. Blackstone, J. and Josipovic, Z. 1986. Zen for Beginners. NewYork:Writers’ and Readers’ Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braver,T.S, Barch, D.M., Keys, B.A., Carter, C.S. Cohen, J. D., Kaye, J. A., 2001. Context processing in older adults: Evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130 (4), 746–763. Broadbent, D. 1958. Perception and Communication. London and NewYork: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K.W. and Ryan, R.M. 2003. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (4), 822–848. Brown, M.W. 1930. Continuous reaction as a measure of attention. Child Development, 1, 255–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleary, T. (trans.) 1995. Minding Mind: A Course in Basic Meditation (especially Chapter 4: Zen Master Dogen:A generally recommended mode of sitting meditation, pp. 22–26. Boston, MA: Shambhala. Cohen, R.M., Semple, W.E., Gross, M., Holcomb, H.J., Dowling, S.M., and Nordahl, T.E. 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Functional localization of sustained attention. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 3–20. Collins, A.M. & Loftus, E.F. 1975. A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. PsychologicalReview, 82, 407–428. Corbetta, M. 1998. Fronto-parietal cortical networds for directing attention and the eye to visual loca tions: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 95, 831–838. D’Aquili, E. & Newberg, A.B. 1999. The Mystical Mind: Probing the Biology of Religious Experience Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R.J., Kabat-Zirm, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., et al. 2003. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deikman,A.J. 1982. The Observing Self. Boston: Beacon Press. Dement,W. 1999. The Promise of Sleep. NewYork: Dell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R. and Duncan, J. 1995. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review ofNeuroscience, 18, 193–222. Duffy, E. 1962. Activation and Behavior. NewYork:Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G.M. 1989. The Remembered Present. NewYork: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, M. 1995. Thoughts Without a Thinker: Psychotherapy from a Buddhist perspective. New York: Basic Books. Flavell, J.H. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J.M. 1989. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe. NewYork: Raven Press. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. 1987. Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by repre

    Google Scholar 

  • sentational memory, in F. Plum and V. Mountcastle (eds.), Handbook of Physiology—The Nervous System, vol. 5. Behesda, MD:American Physiological Society, pp. 373–417. Hanh,T.N 1974. Zen Keys. NewYork, NY: Double Day & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • — 1976. The Miracle of Mindfulness. Boston: Beacon Press. — 1998. The Heart of the Buddha’s Teachings. Berkeley, CA: Parallax Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, W. 1986. The psychology of attention, in J.E. LeDoux and W. Hirst (eds.), Mind and Brain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 105–141. Hirst, W., Spelke, E.S., Reaves, C.C., Caharack, G., and Neisser, U. 1980. Dividing attention without alternation or automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 98–117. Huber, C. 2003. WhenYou’re Falling, Dive:Acceptance, Freedom and Possibility. Murphys, California: Keep it

    Google Scholar 

  • Simple Books. Humphreys, C. 1971. A Western Approach to Zen.Wheaton, IL:The Theosophical Publishing House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, M.S. and Revelle,W. 1984. Personality, motivation, and performance:A theory of the relationship between individual difference and information processing. Psychological Review, 91, 153–184.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H.T. 1995. On the Nature of Consciousness: Cognitive, Phenomenological, and Transpersonal Perspectives. New Haven: Yale University Press. James,W. 1890/1950. The Principles of Psychology, vol. 1. NewYork: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • John, E.R. 2003.A theory of consciousness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(6), 244–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, W.A. and Heinz, S.P. 1975. Depth of non-target processing in an attention task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5, 168–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • —1978. Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 420–435. Kahneman, D. 1973. Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S. 1987. Cited in: The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kapleau, P. (ed.) 1965. The Three Pillars of Zen:Teaching, Practice, and Enlightenment. Boston: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, B. and Whishaw, I.Q. 2003. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (especially pp. 384–385). NewYork, NY:Worth Publishers. LaBerge, D. 1975.Acquisition of automatic processing in perceptual and associative learning, in P.M.A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbit and S. Dornic (eds.), Attention and Performance V. London:Academic Press, pp. 50–64. —1995.Attentional Processing:The Brain’s Art of Mindfulness.Cambridge,MA:Harvard University Press. Langer, E.J. 1989. Mindfulness. Reading, MA:Addison Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • —2000. Mindful learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9 (6), 220–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. and Cox, S. 1997. On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8(5), 395–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. and Fox, E. 2000. The role of perceptual load in negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1038–1052. Mackworth, N.H. 1950. Researches on the measurement of human performance. Medical ResearchCouncil Special Report Series 268. London: His Majesty’s Stationery Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. (March/April, 1997). Is your attention under your control? The diabolic Stroop effect. Psychological Science Agenda, 6–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Davies, D.R., and Less, J.L. 1990. Arousal, extraversion, and individual differences in resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 150–168. Metcalfe, J. and Shimamura, A.P. (eds.) 1994. Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing. Cambridge, MA:

    Google Scholar 

  • MIT Press. Miller, E.K. 2000.The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 59–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monk,T.H. 1991. Sleep, Sleepiness and Performance. NewYork, NY: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, J.H. and Foote, S.L. 1986 Noradrenergic and serotonergic innervation or cortical, thalamic and tectal structures in old and new world monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 143, 117–118. Motter, B.C. 1998. Neurophysiology of visual attention, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain

    Google Scholar 

  • Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 51–70. Neibur, E. and Koch, C. 1998. Computational architectures for attention, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), TheAttentive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 163–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson,T.O. 1992. Metacognition: Core Readings. Boston, MA:Allyn and Bacon. Nelson, T.O. and Narens, L. 1990. Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings, in G.H.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 26 New York: Academic Press, pp. 125–173. Newman, J. and Baars, B. 1993.A neural attentional model for access to consciousness:A global work space perspective. Concepts in Neuroscience, 4 (2), 255–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novak, P. (Summer 1990).The practice of attention. Parabola, 5–12. Okumura, S. (trans.) 1985. Shikantaza:An Introduction to Zazen.Tokyo, Japan: Sotoshu Shomuchu.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R.1998. The attentive brain: Issues and prospects, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The AttentiveBrain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–15. Parasuraman, R.,Wark, J.S., and See, J.E. 1998. Brain systems of vigilance, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The

    Google Scholar 

  • Attentive Brain, Chapter 11. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 221–256. Posner, M.I. and Boies, S. J. 1971. Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78, 391–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. and DiGirolamo, G.J. 1998. Executive attention: Conflict, target detection, and cognitive control, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 401–423. — 2000. Cognitive neuroscience: Origins and promise. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (6), 873–889.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. and Peterson, S.E. 1990.The attention system in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. Posner, M.I. and Rothbart, M.K. 1991. Attentional mechanisms and conscious experience, in

    Google Scholar 

  • Millner, A.O. and Rugg, M.O. (eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness, Chapter 5. New York: Academic Press, pp. 92–111. Rafal, R.D. 1996. Visual attention: Converging operations from neurology and psychology, in

    Google Scholar 

  • A.F Kramer, M.G.H. Coles and G.D. Logal (eds.), Converging Operations in the Study of Visual Selective Attention.Washington:American Psychological Association, pp. 139–192. Rani, N.J. and Rao, P.V.K. 1986. Meditation and attention regulation. Journal of Indian Psychology, 14 (1,2), 26–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rees, G., Kreiman, G., and Koch, C. 2002. Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Neuroscience,3, 261–270. Robbins, T. 1998. Arousal and attention: Psychopharmacological and neuropsychological studies in experimental animals, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 198–220. Rosch, E. 1997. Mindfulness meditation and the private (?) self. In U. Neisser and D. Jopling (eds.), Culture, Experience, and the Conceptual Self. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 185–202. ———1999. Is wisdom in the brain? Psychological Science, 10 (3), 222–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, E. 2004. Mindfulness and consumerism, in T. Kasser and A.D. Kanner (eds.), Psychology and Consumer Culture. Washington, DC:APA Books, pp. 107–125. Schneider, W. and Shiffrin, R.M. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing:

    Google Scholar 

  • Detection, search and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66. Shapiro, S.L., Schwartz, G.E., and Bonner, G. 1998. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 581–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R.M. and Schneider, W. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II, Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review 84, 127–190. Solms, M. and Turnbull, O. 2002. The Brain and the Inner World: An Introduction to the Neuroscience ofSubjective Experience. NewYork, NY: Other Press. Solso, R.L. 2001. Cognitive Psychology, 6th ed. Boston, MA:Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solso, R.L., Maclin, M. K., and Maclin, O. H. 2005. Cognitive Psychology, 7th ed. Boston, MA: Allyn &

    Google Scholar 

  • Bacon. Spelke, E., Hirst,W., and Neisser, U. 1976. Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J.R. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. Suzuki, S. 1962. The Essentials of Zen Buddhism. NewYork: E. P. Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • — 1970. Zen Mind, Beginner’s Mind. NewYork:Weatherhill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teichner, W.H. 1974. The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time on watch. Human Factors, 16, 339–353. Tipper, S.P. 1985.The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journalof Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590. Tomita, H., Ohbayashi, M., Nakahara, K., Hasegawa, I., and Miyashita,Y. 1999.Top–down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval. Nature, 401, 699–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. and Gelade, G. 1980. A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. Umitla, C. 1988. The control operations of consciousness, in A.J. Marcel and E. Bisiach (eds.), Consciousness and Contemporary Science. NewYork: Oxford University Press, pp. 334–355. —2000.Conscious experience depends on multiple brain systems.European Psychologist,5 (1),3–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood,T. 1997. On knowing what you know: Metacognition and the act of reading. The ClearingHouse, 71(2), 77–80. Valentine, E.R. and Sweet, P.L. 1999. Meditation and attention: A comparison of the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 2, 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Zomeren, A.H. and Brouwer, W.H. 1994. Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Oxford University Press. Varela, F.J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. 1991. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and HumanExperience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verfaellie, M., Bowers, D., and Heilman, K. 1988. Hemispheric asymmetries in mediating intention, but not selective attention. Neuropsychologia, 26, 521–531. [Cited by Austin 1998, re: point that attention is different from intention and where intention is defined as: “(normal), deliberate, anticipatory preparation for action,” p. 275.] Wallace,A. 1991.The Buddhist tradition of Samatha: Methods for refining and examining consciousness.Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2–3), 175–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, R. and Vaughn, F. (eds.) 1993. Paths Beyond Ego:The Transpersonal Vision (especially pp. 47–55). NewYork:Tarcher-Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins,A., Shallice,T., and McCarthy, R. 1987. Frontal lesions and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 359–365. [Cited by Austin 1998, re: claim that points with frontal lobe damage have difficulty focusing in a boring, monotonous task.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, D. and Knight, R. 1986. Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility after dorsolateral prefontral lesions. Neurology, 36, 212–216. [Cited by Austin 1998.] Zaleski, P. and Kaufman, P. 1997. Gifts of the Spirit: Living theWisdom of the Great Religious Traditions (espe cially pp. 1–11). NewYork: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Kelly Bulkeley (Visiting Scholar)

Copyright information

© 2005 Kelly Bulkeley

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kahan, T.L., Simone, P.M. (2005). Where Neurocognition Meets the Master:Attention and Metacognition in Zen. In: Bulkeley, K. (eds) Soul, Psyche, Brain: New Directions in the Study of Religion and Brain-Mind Science. Palgrave Macmillan, New York. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781403979230_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics