Abstract

Given that religion is a human universal,evolutionary and cultural perspectives on the emotions are incomplete without a comprehensive understanding of the role of religion. Religion provides context and direction for emotion and the influence of religious systems on emotional experience and expression is considerable. For example, religions prescribe certain emotions and proscribe others. Religion also modulates the expression of emotion.This chapter will examine emotions and emotional processes that normally occur in the context of religion. Recent scientific research on religion and emotion will be highlighted.

Keywords

Depression Dopamine Cortisol Posit Trench 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abdullaev, Y.G. and Posner, M.I. 1997. Time course of activating brain areas in generating verbal associations. Psychological Science, 8, 56–59.Google Scholar
  2. Aitken, R. 1982. Taking the Path of Zen. New York: North Point Press.Google Scholar
  3. Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  4. Baars, B. 1988. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. —. 1997. In the Theater of Consciousness:The Workspace of the Mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. 1987. Human Memory:Theory and Practice. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bargh, J. and Chartrand, T. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.Google Scholar
  8. Blackmore, S. 2004. Consciousness:An Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Blackstone, J. and Josipovic, Z. 1986. Zen for Beginners. New York:Writers’ and Readers’ Publishing.Google Scholar
  10. Braver, T.S, Barch, D.M., Keys, B.A., Carter, C.S. Cohen, J. D., Kaye, J. A., 2001. Context processing in older adults: Evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130 (4), 746–763.Google Scholar
  11. Broadbent, D. 1958. Perception and Communication. London and New York: Pergamon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brown, K.W. and Ryan, R.M. 2003. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (4), 822–848.Google Scholar
  13. Brown, M.W. 1930. Continuous reaction as a measure of attention. Child Development, 1, 255–291.Google Scholar
  14. Cleary, T. (trans.) 1995. Minding Mind: A Course in Basic Meditation(especially Chapter 4: Zen Master Dogen:A generally recommended mode of sitting meditation, pp. 22–26. Boston, MA: Shambhala.Google Scholar
  15. Cohen, R.M., Semple, W.E., Gross, M., Holcomb, H.J., Dowling, S.M., and Nordahl, T.E. 1988.Google Scholar
  16. Functional localization of sustained attention. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 3–20.Google Scholar
  17. Collins, A.M. & Loftus, E.F. 1975. A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.Google Scholar
  18. Corbetta, M. 1998. Fronto-parietal cortical networds for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 95, 831–838.Google Scholar
  19. D’Aquili, E. & Newberg, A.B. 1999. The Mystical Mind: Probing the Biology of Religious Experience. Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press.Google Scholar
  20. Davidson, R.J., Kabat-Zirm, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., et al. 2003. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564–570.Google Scholar
  21. Deikman, A.J. 1982. The Observing Self. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
  22. Dement, W. 1999. The Promise of Sleep. New York: Dell Publishing.Google Scholar
  23. Desimone, R. and Duncan, J. 1995. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.Google Scholar
  24. Duffy, E. 1962. Activation and Behavior. New York:Wiley.Google Scholar
  25. Edelman, G.M. 1989. The Remembered Present. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  26. Epstein, M. 1995. Thoughts Without a Thinker: Psychotherapy from a Buddhist perspective. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  27. Flavell, J.H. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.Google Scholar
  28. Fuster, J.M. 1989. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe. NewYork: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  29. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. 1987. Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational memory, in F. Plum and V. Mountcastle (eds.), Handbook of Physiology—The Nervous System, vol. 5. Behesda, MD:American Physiological Society, pp. 373–417.Google Scholar
  30. Hanh, T.N 1974. Zen Keys. New York, NY: Double Day & Co.Google Scholar
  31. —. 1976. The Miracle of Mindfulness. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
  32. —. 1998. The Heart of the Buddha’s Teachings. Berkeley, CA: Parallax Press.Google Scholar
  33. Hirst, W. 1986. The psychology of attention, in J.E. LeDoux and W. Hirst (eds.), Mind and Brain. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 105–141.Google Scholar
  34. Hirst, W., Spelke, E.S., Reaves, C.C., Caharack, G., and Neisser, U. 1980. Dividing attention without alternation or automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 98–117.Google Scholar
  35. Huber, C. 2003. When You’re Falling, Dive:Acceptance, Freedom and Possibility. Murphys, California: Keep it Simple Books.Google Scholar
  36. Humphreys, C. 1971. A Western Approach to Zen.Wheaton, IL:The Theosophical Publishing House.Google Scholar
  37. Humphreys, M.S. and Revelle, W. 1984. Personality, motivation, and performance:A theory of the relationship between individual difference and information processing. Psychological Review, 91, 153–184.Google Scholar
  38. Hunt, H.T. 1995. On the Nature of Consciousness: Cognitive, Phenomenological, and Transpersonal Perspectives. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  39. James, W. 1890/1950. The Principles of Psychology, vol. 1. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
  40. John, E.R. 2003.A theory of consciousness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(6), 244–250.Google Scholar
  41. Johnston, W.A. and Heinz, S.P. 1975. Depth of non-target processing in an attention task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5, 168–175.Google Scholar
  42. —. 1978. Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 420–435.Google Scholar
  43. Kahneman, D. 1973. Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  44. Kaplan S. 1987. Cited in: The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Kapleau, P. (ed.) 1965. The Three Pillars of Zen:Teaching, Practice, and Enlightenment. Boston: Beacon Press. Kolb, B. and Whishaw, I.Q. 2003. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology(especially pp. 384–385). NewYork, NY:Worth Publishers.Google Scholar
  46. LaBerge, D. 1975. Acquisition of automatic processing in perceptual and associative learning, in P.M.A. Rabbit and S. Dornic (eds.), Attention and Performance V. London:Academic Press, pp. 50–64.Google Scholar
  47. —. 1995. Attentional Processing:The Brain’s Art of Mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Langer, E.J. 1989. Mindfulness. Reading, MA:Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
  48. —. 2000. Mindful learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9 (6), 220–223.Google Scholar
  49. Lavie, N. and Cox, S. 1997. On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8(5), 395–398.Google Scholar
  50. Lavie, N. and Fox, E. 2000. The role of perceptual load in negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1038–1052.Google Scholar
  51. Mackworth, N.H. 1950. Researches on the measurement of human performance. Medical Research Council Special Report Series 268. London: His Majesty’s Stationery Office.Google Scholar
  52. MacLeod, C. (March/April, 1997). Is your attention under your control? The diabolic Stroop effect. Psychological Science Agenda, 6–7.Google Scholar
  53. Matthews, G., Davies, D.R., and Less, J.L. 1990. Arousal, extraversion, and individual differences in resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 150–168.Google Scholar
  54. Metcalfe, J. and Shimamura, A.P. (eds.) 1994. Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  55. Miller, E.K. 2000.The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 59–65.Google Scholar
  56. Monk, T.H. 1991. Sleep, Sleepiness and Performance. New York, NY: John Wiley.Google Scholar
  57. Morrison, J.H. and Foote, S.L. 1986 Noradrenergic and serotonergic innervation or cortical, thalamic and tectal structures in old and new world monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 143, 117–118.Google Scholar
  58. Motter, B.C. 1998. Neurophysiology of visual attention, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive BrainCambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 51–70.Google Scholar
  59. Neibur, E. and Koch, C. 1998. Computational architectures for attention, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 163–186.Google Scholar
  60. Nelson, T.O. 1992. Metacognition: Core Readings. Boston, MA:Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
  61. Nelson, T.O. and Narens, L. 1990. Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings, in G.H. Bower (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 26 New York: Academic Press, pp. 125–173.Google Scholar
  62. Newman, J. and Baars, B. 1993. A neural attentional model for access to consciousness: A global workspace perspective. Concepts in Neuroscience, 4 (2), 255–290.Google Scholar
  63. Novak, P. (Summer 1990).The practice of attention. Parabola, 5–12.Google Scholar
  64. Okumura, S. (trans.) 1985. Shikantaza:An Introduction to Zazen.Tokyo, Japan: Sotoshu Shomuchu.Google Scholar
  65. Parasuraman, R.1998. The attentive brain: Issues and prospects, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–15.Google Scholar
  66. Parasuraman, R., Wark, J.S., and See, J.E. 1998. Brain systems of vigilance, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain, Chapter 11. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 221–256.Google Scholar
  67. Posner, M.I. and Boies, S. J. 1971. Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78, 391–408.Google Scholar
  68. Posner, M.I. and DiGirolamo, G.J. 1998. Executive attention: Conflict, target detection, and cognitive control, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 401–423.Google Scholar
  69. —. 2000. Cognitive neuroscience: Origins and promise. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (6), 873–889.Google Scholar
  70. Posner, M.I. and Peterson, S.E. 1990.The attention system in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.Google Scholar
  71. Posner, M.I. and Rothbart, M.K. 1991. Attentional mechanisms and conscious experience, in Millner, A.O. and Rugg, M.O. (eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness, Chapter 5. New York: Academic Press, pp. 92–111.Google Scholar
  72. Rafal, R.D. 1996. Visual attention: Converging operations from neurology and psychology, in A.F Kramer, M.G.H. Coles and G.D. Logal (eds.), Converging Operations in the Study of Visual Selective Attention.Washington: American Psychological Association, pp. 139–192.Google Scholar
  73. Rani, N.J. and Rao, P.V.K. 1986. Meditation and attention regulation. Journal of Indian Psychology, 14 (1, 2), 26–30.Google Scholar
  74. Rees, G., Kreiman, G., and Koch, C. 2002. Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Neuroscience, 3, 261–270.Google Scholar
  75. Robbins, T. 1998. Arousal and attention: Psychopharmacological and neuropsychological studies in experimental animals, in R. Parasuraman (ed.), The Attentive Brain.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 198–220.Google Scholar
  76. Rosch, E. 1997. Mindfulness meditation and the private (?) self. In U. Neisser and D. Jopling (eds.), Culture, Experience, and the Conceptual Self. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 185–202.Google Scholar
  77. —1999. Is wisdom in the brain? Psychological Science, 10 (3), 222–224.Google Scholar
  78. Rosenberg, E. 2004. Mindfulness and consumerism, in T. Kasser and A.D. Kanner (eds.), Psychology and Consumer Culture.Washington, DC:APA Books, pp. 107–125.Google Scholar
  79. Schneider, W. and Shiffrin, R.M. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: Detection, search and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.Google Scholar
  80. Shapiro, S.L., Schwartz, G.E., and Bonner, G. 1998. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 581–599.Google Scholar
  81. Shiffrin, R.M. and Schneider, W. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II, Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review84, 127–190.Google Scholar
  82. Solms, M. and Turnbull, O. 2002. The Brain and the Inner World: An Introduction to the Neuroscience of Subjective Experience. New York, NY: Other Press.Google Scholar
  83. Solso, R.L. 2001. Cognitive Psychology, 6th ed. Boston, MA:Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
  84. Solso, R.L., Maclin, M. K., and Maclin, O. H. 2005. Cognitive Psychology, 7th ed. Boston, MA:Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  85. Spelke, E., Hirst, W., and Neisser, U. 1976. Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–230.Google Scholar
  86. Stroop, J.R. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.Google Scholar
  87. Suzuki, S. 1962. The Essentials of Zen Buddhism. New York: E. P. Dutton.Google Scholar
  88. —. 1970. Zen Mind, Beginner’s Mind. New York:Weatherhill.Google Scholar
  89. Teichner, W.H. 1974. The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time on watch. Human Factors, 16, 339–353.Google Scholar
  90. Tipper, S.P. 1985.The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.Google Scholar
  91. Tomita, H., Ohbayashi, M., Nakahara, K., Hasegawa, I., and Miyashita, Y. 1999.Top–down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval. Nature, 401, 699–703.Google Scholar
  92. Treisman, A. and Gelade, G. 1980. A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.Google Scholar
  93. Umitla, C. 1988. The control operations of consciousness, in A.J. Marcel and E. Bisiach (eds.), Consciousness and Contemporary Science. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 334–355.Google Scholar
  94. —. 2000. Conscious experience depends on multiple brain systems. European Psychologist, 5 (1), 3–11.Google Scholar
  95. Underwood, T. 1997. On knowing what you know: Metacognition and the act of reading. The Clearing House, 71(2), 77–80.Google Scholar
  96. Valentine, E.R. and Sweet, P.L. 1999. Meditation and attention: A comparison of the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 2, 59–70.Google Scholar
  97. Va n Zomeren, A.H. and Brouwer, W.H. 1994. Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  98. Varela, F.J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. 1991. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  99. Verfaellie, M., Bowers, D., and Heilman, K. 1988. Hemispheric asymmetries in mediating intention, but not selective attention. Neuropsychologia, 26, 521–531. [Cited by Austin 1998, re: point that attention is different from intention and where intention is defined as: “(normal), deliberate, anticipatory preparation for action, ” p. 275.]Google Scholar
  100. Wallace, A. 1991.The Buddhist tradition of Samatha: Methods for refining and examining consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2–3), 175–187.Google Scholar
  101. Walsh, R. and Vaughn, F. (eds.) 1993. Paths Beyond Ego: The Transpersonal Vision(especially pp. 47–55). New York:Tarcher-Putnam.Google Scholar
  102. Wilkins, A., Shallice, T., and McCarthy, R. 1987. Frontal lesions and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 359–365. [Cited by Austin 1998, re: claim that points with frontal lobe damage have difficulty focusing in a boring, monotonous task.]Google Scholar
  103. Woods, D. and Knight, R. 1986. Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility after dorsolateral prefontral lesions. Neurology, 36, 212–216. [Cited by Austin 1998.]Google Scholar
  104. Zaleski, P. and Kaufman, P. 1997. Gifts of the Spirit: Living the Wisdom of the Great Religious Traditions(especially pp. 1–11). New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kelly Bulkeley 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert A. Emmons

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations