Advertisement

Flow Experiences: From Mystery to Mechanism

  • Arne Dietrich
Chapter

Abstract

The lead guitarist of R.E.M, Peter Buck, once made the following observation about the band’s most famous song:

When I think about Losing My Religion, I think about the process of writing and recording it, and how dream-like and effortless it was. The music was written in five minutes; the first time the band played it, it fell into place perfectly. Michael had lyrics within the hour, and while playing the song for the third or fourth time, I found myself incredibly moved to hear the vocals in conjunction with the music. To me, Losing My Religion feels like some kind of archetype that was floating around in space that we managed to lasso. If only all song writing was this easy.

Keywords

Prefrontal Cortex Flow Experience Implicit Knowledge Efference Copy Spontaneous Mode 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Recommended readings

  1. Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1281–1289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D., & Frith, C. (2000). Why can’t you tickle yourself? NeuroReport, 11, 11–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Crick, R H. C, & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 97–107.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity. New York: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
  5. Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 735–808.Google Scholar
  6. Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 746–761.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dietrich, A., & Audiffren, M. (2011). The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1305–1325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dietrich, A., & Haider, H. (2015). Human creativity, evolutionary algorithms, and predictive representations: The mechanics of thought trials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1011–1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Downing, K. L. (2009). Predictive models in the brain. Connection Science, 21, 39–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Limb, C., & Braun, A. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS ONE, 3, el679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: Mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1273–1280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Arne Dietrich 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Arne Dietrich
    • 1
  1. 1.American University of BeirutLebanon

Personalised recommendations