Skip to main content

Beyond Facial Expression: Spatial Distance as a Factor in the Communication of Discrete Emotions

  • Chapter
Book cover The Social Psychology of Nonverbal Communication
  • 966 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter examines the role of nonverbal behavior other than facial expressions in displaying and communicating emotion. Our thesis is that a wider variety of displays than are often considered are responsible for communicating a broad assortment of specific emotions, and that one of the primary variables important in this is the spatial distance in the ecology of an organism at which a given emotion is typically displayed. The emotions most associated with facial expressions — the classic primary affects including at least happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, and disgust — best function as displays at moderate personal distances. At intimate distances, a variety of emotions including love, lust, gratitude, anger, and sympathy can be reliably communicated through displays involving pheromones and touch. At longer social and public distances, social and moral emotions including pride/ arrogance/triumph, guilt/shame/humiliation, envy/jealousy/resentment, and pity/scorn/contempt are communicated through larger and more substantial body postures and gestures associated with ancient dominance and submission displays. Finally, the GREAT emotions (gratitude, respect, elevation, appreciation, and trust) are signaled at a dyadic level via mutually contingent responsiveness and interpersonal synchrony.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1968). The historical background of modern social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., pp. 1–80). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, P. A. (1998). The cognitive valence theory of intimate communication. In M. Palmer & G. Barnett (Eds.), Progress in communication sciences vol. XIV. Mutual influence in interpersonal communication: Theory and research in cognition, affect, and behavior (pp. 39–72). Stamford, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • App, B., McIntosh, D., Reed, C., & Hertenstein, M. (2011). Nonverbal channel use in communication of emotion: How may depend on why. Emotion, 11, 603–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baxter, L. A. (1992). Forms and functions of intimate play in personal relationships. Human Communication Research, 18, 336–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berglund, H., Lindstrom, P., & Savic, I. (2006). Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 8269–8274. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600331103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1980). Nonverbal behavior and the theory of emotion: The facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(5), 811–824. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.38.5.811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(3), 389–413. doi: 10.1037/0033–295X.92.3.389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1993). Emotional communication, emotional competence, and physical illness: A developmental-interactionist view. In H. Traue & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Emotional expressiveness, inhibition, and health (pp. 32–56). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe and Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1999). The biological affects: A typology. Psychological Review, 106(2), 301–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (2004). The gratitude of exchange and the gratitude of caring: A devel- opmental-interactionist perspective of moral emotion. In R. A. Emmons & M. McCullough (Eds.), The psychology of gratitude (pp. 100–122). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (2014). Emotion: A biosocial synthesis. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., & Boone, R. T. (2009a). Internalizer. In D. Sander & K. Scherer (Eds.), Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences (p. 222). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., & Boone, R. T. (2009b). Externalizer. In D. Sander & K. Scherer (Eds.), Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences (p. 169). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J., & Hale, J. (1988). Nonverbal expectancy violations: Model elaboration and application to immediacy behaviors. Communication Monographs, 55(1), 58–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J., Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L. (1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic interaction patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1991). The biological origins of automated patterns of human interaction. Communication Theory, 1, 4–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, D., & Haviland-Jones, J. (1999). Rapid mood change and human odors. Physiology & Behavior, 68, 241–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clynes, M. (1977). Sentics, the touch of the emotions. New York: Anchor Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clynes, M. (1988). Generalized emotion, how it is produced and sentic cycle therapy. In M. Clynes & J. Panksepp (Eds.), Emotions and psychopathology (pp. 107–170). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1966). Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143, 338–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1967). A segmentation of behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 5, 221–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Contreras, C. M., Gutierrez-Garcia, A. G., Mendoza-Lopez, R., Rodriguez-Landa, J. F., Bernal-Morales, B., & Diaz-Marte, C. (2013). Amniotic fluid elicits appetitive responses in human newborns: Fatty acids and appetitive responses. Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 221–231. doi: 10.1002/dev.21012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dagg, A. (2011). Animal friendships. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1998). The expression of the emotions in man and animals (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press (Original work published 1872).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, T. (2012). “Emotion”: The history of a keyword in crisis. Emotion Review, 4, 338–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioral function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, V.W. (1978). Facial Action Coding System (FACS): A technique for the measurement of facial action (pp. 19–46). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, S. (Writer), & Fleming, S. K. (Director). (2012). Animal Odd Couples [Television series episode]. In F. Kaufman (Executive producer), Nature. New York: Public Broadcasting System.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164(3875), 86–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (2002). Altruism in competitive environments. In R. J. Davidson & A. Harrington (Eds.), Visions of compassion (pp. 182–211). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frasnelli, J., Lundström, J. N., Boyle, J. A., Katsarkas, A., & Jones-Gotmann, M. (2011). The vomeronasal organ is not involved in the perception of endogenous odors. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 450–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., Coupland, J., & Coupland, N. (1991). Contexts of accommodation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M., Keltner, D., App, B., Bulleit, A., & Jaskolka, A. (2006). Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion, 6, 528–533. doi: 10.1037/1528–3542.6.3.528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M., Holmes, R., Keltner, D., & McCullough, M. (2009). The communication of emotion via touch. Emotion, 9, 566–573. doi: 10.1037/a0016108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Homans, G. C. (1966). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1933). Hypnosis and suggestibility. New York: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummer, T. A., & McClintock, M. K. (2009). Putative human pheromone andro- stadienone attunes the mind specifically to emotional information. Hormones and Behavior, 55, 548–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Horberg, E. J., & Oveis, C. (2006). Emotional intuitions and moral play. Social Justice Research, 19, 208–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1970). Movement coordination in social interaction: Some examples described. Acta Psychologica, 32, 100–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of motivation definitions, with a suggestion for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5(3), 263–291. doi: 10.1007/BF00993889.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohl, J. V., Atzmueller, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Human pheromones: integrating neuroendocrinology and ethology. Neuro Endocrinological Letters, 22, 309–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohl, J. V., & Francoeur, R. T. (2012). The scent of eros: Mysteries of odor in human sexuality [E-book]. Retrieved from http://bookstore.iuniverse.com/Products/SKU-000574070/The-Scent-of-Eros.aspx.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone, M. S., Harris-Warrick, R. M., & Kravitz, E. A. (1980). Serotonin and octopamine produce opposite postures in lobsters. Science, 208(4439), 76–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loumaye, E., Thorner, J., & Catt, K. J. (1982). Yeast mating pheromone activates mammalian gonadotropins: Evolutionary conservation of a reproductive hormone? Science, 218(4579), 1323–1325. doi: 10.1126/science.6293058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J., Sterck, E., & de Vos, H. (2010). Close social association in animals and humans: functions and mechanisms of friendship. Behaviour, 147, 1379–1412. doi: 10.1163/000579510X528224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mast, T. G., & Samuelsen, C. L. (2009). Human pheromone detection by the vomeronasal organ: Unnecessary for mate selection? Chemical Senses, 34, 529–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2012). Evidence for a nonverbal expression of triumph. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 520–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClintock, M. K. (1971). Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature, 229(5282), 244–245. doi: 10.1038/229244a0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, R. P., Bonsall, R. W., & Kutner, M. (1975). Volatile fatty acids, “copu- lins”, in human vaginal secretions. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1, 153–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, I., Löken, L., & Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 305–314. doi: 10.1007/s00221–009-2007-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemiah, J. C., & Sifneos, P. E. (1970). Psychosomatic illness: Problem in communication. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 18, 154–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Day, D. H., & Horgen, P. A. (1981). Sexual interactions in eukaryotic microbes. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ober, C., Weitkamp, L. R., Cox, N., Dytch, H., Kostyu, D., & Elias, S. (1997). HLA and mate choice in humans. American Journal of Human Genetics, 61, 497–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy. Psychological Review, 83, 231–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: A functionalist perspective. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. (1991). A longitudinal study of popular and rejected children’s rough- and-tumble play. Early Education & Development, 2, 205–213. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed0203_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pert, C. B. (1997). Molecules of emotion: Why you feel the way you feel. New York: Scribner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, S. R., Rauh, C., Buck, R., Henning, R., & West, T. V. (2011). The effect of video feedback delay on frustration and emotion communication accuracy. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1651–1657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinn, W. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial expression: A review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 52–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roloff, M. E., & Campion, D. E. (1985). Conversational profit-seeking: Interaction as social exchange. In R. L. Street, Jr. & J. N. Cappella (Eds.), Sequence and pattern in communicative behavior (pp. 161–189). London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaffner C. M., & Aureli F. (2005). Embraces and grooming in captive spider monkeys. International Journal of Primatology, 26, 1093–1106. doi: 10.1007/s10764–005-6460–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sober, E. (2002). Kindness and cruelty in evolution. In R. J. Davidson & A. Harrington (Eds.), Visions of compassion: Western scientists and Tibetan Buddhists examine human nature (pp. 46–65). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. S. (1962–1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness (Vols. 1 & 2). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous display of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 11655–11660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 103–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyart, C., Webster, W. W., Chen, J. H., Wilson, S. R., McClary, A., Khan, R. M., & Sobel, N. (2007). Smelling a single component of male sweat alters levels of cortisol in women. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1261–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wysocki, C. J., & Preti, G. (2004). Facts, fallacies, fears, and frustrations with human pheromones. The Anatomical Record, 281A(1), 1201–1211. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2015 Ross Buck and Mike Miller

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Buck, R., Miller, M. (2015). Beyond Facial Expression: Spatial Distance as a Factor in the Communication of Discrete Emotions. In: Kostić, A., Chadee, D. (eds) The Social Psychology of Nonverbal Communication. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137345868_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics