Teamwork pp 56-77 | Cite as

Cooperation, Risk and the Evolution of Teamwork

  • Peter Andras
  • John Lazarus

Abstract

Our aims in this chapter are twofold. First, we place teamwork in the context of the evolutionary analysis of cooperation and altruism. This allows us to predict the evolutionary scenarios likely to have favoured the evolution of team work, the probable origins of human teamwork and the biases to be predicted in team thinking. Second, we examine the influences of environmental adversity and uncertainty (both conceptualized as ‘risk’) on cooperation in the organic world and describe a new model to explain these influences. We conclude by drawing conclusions about the role of environmental risk in the emergence of human cooperation and teamwork.

Keywords

Biomass Expense Pseudomonas Hunt Lution 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andras, P., Lazarus, J. and Roberts, G. (In prep.) Environmental adversity and uncertainty favour cooperation. Mimeo. Google Scholar
  2. Andras, P., Roberts, G. and Lazarus, J. (2003) Environmental risk, cooperation and communication complexity. In: Adaptive Agents and Multi-Agent Systems (edited by Alonso, E., Kudenko, D. and Kazakov, D.), pp. 49–65. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Axelrod, R. (1984) The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  4. Axelrod, R. and Hamilton, W. (1981) ‘The evolution of cooperation’. Science, 211, 1390–1396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1992) (eds) The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Boyd, R. and Lorberbaum, J. P. (1987) ‘No pure strategy is evolutionarily stable in the repeated Prisoner’s Dilemma game’, Nature, 327, 58–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Callaway, R., Brooker, R., Choler, P., Kikvidze, Z., Lortiek, C., Michalet, R., Paolini, L., Pugnaireq, F., Newingham, B., Aschehoug, E., Armasq, C., Kikodze, D. and Cook, B. (2002) ‘Positive interactions among alpine plants increase with stress’, Nature, 417, 844–847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Caporael, L. R., Dawes, R. M., Orbell, J. M. and van de Kragt, A. J. C. (1989) ‘Selfishness examined: Cooperation in the absence of egoistic incentives’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 683–739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Colman, A. M. (2003) ‘Cooperation, psychological game theory, and limitations of rationality in social interaction’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 139–198.Google Scholar
  10. Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (2002) ‘Two special issues on Risk’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 1–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Darwin, C. (1872) The origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life (6th edn). London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  12. de Bono, M., Tobin, D. M., Davis, M. W., Avery, L. and Bargmann, C. I. (2002) ‘Social feeding in Caenorhabditis eiegans is induced by neurons that detect aversive stimuli’, Nature, 419, 899–903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Drenkard, E. and Ausubel, F. M. (2002) ‘Pseudomonas biofilm formation and antibiotic resistance are linked to phenotypic variation’, Nature, 416, 740–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dugatkin, L. A. (1997) Cooperation among animals: An evolutionary perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Evans, J. P. and Magurran, A. E. (1999) ‘Geographic variation in sperm production by Trinidadian guppies’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 266, 2083–2087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Farr, J. A. (1975) ‘The role of predation in the evolution of social behavior of natural populations of the guppy, Poecilia reticulata (Pisces: Poeciliidea)’, Evolution, 29, 151–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fehr, E. and Fischbacher, U. (2003) ‘The nature of human altruism’, Nature, 425, 785–791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fehr, E. and Rockenbach, B. (2003) ‘Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism’, Nature, 422, 137–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Frank, R. H. (1988) Passions Within Reason: The Strategic Role of the Emotions. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  20. Frank, R. H., Gilovich, T. and Regan, D. T. (1993) ‘The evolution of one-shot cooperation: an experiment’, Ethnology and Sociobiology, 14, 247–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fukuyama, F. (1996) Human nature and the reconstruction of social order. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  22. Gigerenzer, G. (2002) Reckoning with risk: Learning to live with uncertainty. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
  23. Gintis, H., Smith, E. and Bowles, S. (2001) ‘Costly signaling and cooperation’, J Theor Biol, 213, 103–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Greenberg, E. P. (2003) ‘Tiny teamwork’, Nature, 424, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hamilton, W. D. (1964) ‘The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I and II’, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–16, 17–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F. and Blurton-Jones, N. G. (2001) ‘Hadza meat sharing’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 113–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hogg, M. A. (1992) The social psychology of group cohesiveness. From attraction to social identity. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
  28. Kameda, T., Takezawa, M., Tindale, R. and Smith, C. (2002) ‘Social sharing and risk reduction: Exploring a computational algorithm for the psychology of windfall gains’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 11–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kaplan, H. and Hill, K. (1985) ‘Food sharing among Ache foragers — Tests of explanatory hypotheses’. Current Anthropology, 26, 223–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lazarus, J. (2003) ‘Let’s cooperate to understand cooperation’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 169–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Maynard Smith, J. (1964) ‘Group selection and kin selection’, Nature, 201, 1145–1147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Maynard Smith, J. (1976) ‘Group selection’, Quarterly Review of Biology, 51, 277–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Maynard Smith, J. (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McFarland, D. J. and Lloyd, I. (1973) ‘Time-shared feeding and drinking’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 48–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Milinksi, M., Semmann, D., Bakker, T. and Krambeck, H.-J. (2001) ‘Cooperation through indirect reciprocity: image scoring or standing strategy?’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 268, 2495–2501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Milinski, M., Semmann, D. and Krambeck, H.-J. (2002) ‘Reputation helps solve the “tragedy of the commons”’ Nature, 415, 424–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. O’Toole, G. A. (2002) ‘A resistance switch’, Nature, 416, 695–696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ostrom, E. (1990) Governing the Commons: The Evolution of Institutions for Collective Action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ostrom, E., Walker, J. and Gardner, R. (1992) ‘Covenants with and without a sword: self-governance is possible’, American Political Science Review, 86, 404–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Roberts, G. (1998) ‘Competitive altruism: from reciprocity to the handicap principle’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 265, 427–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Roberts, G. and Sherratt, T. N. (1998) ‘Development of cooperative relationships through increasing investment’, Nature, 394, 175–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Seghers, B. H. (1974) ‘Schooling behaviour in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): an evolutionary response to predation’, Evolution, 28, 486–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Singh, P. K., Parsek, M. R., Greenberg, E. P. and Welsh, M.J. (2002) ‘A component of innate immunity prevents bacterial biofilm development’, Nature, 417, 552–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sober, E. and Wilson, D. S. (1998) Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behaviour. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Sokolowski, M. B. (2002) ‘Social eating for stress’, Nature, 419, 893–894.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Spinks, A. C., Jarvis, J. U. M. and Bennett, N. C. (2000) ‘Comparative patterns of philopatry and dispersal in two common mole-rat populations: implications for the evolution of mole-rat sociality’, Journal of Animal Ecology, 69, 224–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stephens, D. W. and Krebs, J. R. (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Strassmann, J. E., Zhu, Y. and Queller, D. C. (2000) ‘Altruism and social cheating in the social amoeba Dictyostelium discoideum’, Nature, 408, 965–967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tinbergen, N. (1963) ‘On aims and methods of ethology’, Z Tierpsychol, 20, 410–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Trivers, R. L. (1971) ‘The evolution of reciprocal altruism’, The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Trivers, R. L. (1974) ‘Parent-offspring conflict’, American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962) Animal dispersion in relation to social behaviour. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Peter Andras and John Lazarus 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Peter Andras
  • John Lazarus

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations