Skip to main content

So, You had Two Sisters, Right? Functions for Discourse Markers in Alzheimer’s Talk

  • Chapter

Abstract

Naturally occurring conversation with Alzheimer’s speakers, including embedded or co-constructed narrative, can be used to support or augment clinical findings on features of Alzheimer’s discourse, which are as highly variable as is the disease. For example, not only can speakers with moderate to moderately severe Alzheimer’s disease maintain some level of politeness (Sabat & Collins 1999; Temple et al. 1999; Rhys et al. 2000); and interaction (Hamilton 1994a, 1994b; Ramanathan 1997), their pragmatic skills can sustain more fine-tuned analysis, particularly as those skills sustain or simulate fluency. Ellis (1996) comments that problems Alzheimer’s speakers have in organizing and concentrating information are both communicative and cognitive. In the early stages, as grammatical modes of processing deteriorate for whatever reason, those “cohesion ties that structure topicality will begin to fade”; in later stages, more severe problems with maintaining topic will surface (see for example, Kempler 1995). Alzheimer’s speakers typically depend on lexical cohesion, or word-based means of holding the elements of a sentence together, as opposed to grammatical because, adds Ellis, lexical cohesion relies on “meaning,” not on grammatical structures. They lose the ability to “ground” the discourse for the hearer, or to organize meaning with thematic information via pronouns (see Almor et al. 1999)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aijmer, Karin. (1987) “OH and AH in English Conversation.” In Corpus Linguistics and Beyond, Willem Meijs (ed.), Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi, 61–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aijmer, Karin. (2002) English Discourse Particles: Evidence from a Corpus. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Almor, A., Kempler, D., MacDonald, M., Andersen, E. & Tyler, L. (1999) “Why do Alzheimer patients have difficulty with pronouns? Working memory, semantics, and reference in comprehension and production in Alzheimer’s disease.” Brain and Language 67: 202–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberg, Michael. (2004) “Talk, small stories, and adolescent identities”. Human Development 47: 366–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, K. (2003) “Effects of working memory deficits on the communicative functioning of Alzheimer’s dementia patients.” Journal of Communication Disorders 36: 209–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, Diana. (1996) “Are apposition markers discourse markers?” Journal of Linguistics 32: 325–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byron, Donna & Peter A. Heeman. (1997) “Discourse Marker Use in Spoken Dialog.” In Proceedings of the 5th European Conference On Speech Communication and Technology, Rhodes, Greece, September 1997, pp. 2223–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, Herbert. (2002) “Speaking in time.” Speech Communication 36: 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Santi, Susan. (1993) “Formulaic Language in Aging and Alzheimer’s Disease.” PhD dissertation, City University of New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, D. (1996) “Coherence patterns in Alzheimer discourse.” Communication Research 23: 472–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erman, Britt. (2001) “Pragmatic markers revisited with a focus on you know in adult and adolescent talk.” Journal of Pragmatics 33: 1337–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. & Drescher, M. (1996) “Methods for the description of discourse particles: contrastive analysis.” Language Sciences 18: 853–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox Tree, Jean. (2002) “Interpretations of pauses and ums at turn exchanges.” Discourse Processes 34(1): 37–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox Tree, Jean and Josef Schrock. (2002) “Basic meanings of you know and I mean.” Journal of Pragmatics 34: 727–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, Janet. (2003) “Discourse marker use across three contexts: a comparison of native and non-native speaker performance.” Multilingua 22: 185–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, Janet. (2003) “The influence of speaker roles on discourse marker use.” Journal of Pragmatics 35: 23–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosz, Barbara J. & Candace L. Sidner. (1986) “Attention, intention, and the structure of discourse.” Computational Linguistics, 12(3): 175–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guendouzi, Jacqueline & Nicole Muller. (2001) “Intelligibility and rehearsed sequences in conversations with a DAT patient.” Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 15: 91–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunlogson, Christine. (2003) True to Form: Rising and Falling Declaratives as Questions in English. NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunlogson, Christine. (2001) “Rising and falling declaratives.” http://semantic-sarchive.net 2001 07 29; last accessed June 19, 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, Heidi. (1994a) Conversations with an Alzheimer’s Patient Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, Heidi. (1994b) “Requests for Clarification as Evidence of Pragmatic Comprehension Difficulty: The Case of Alzheimer’s Disease.” Pages 185–99 in Bloom, Ronald; Obler, Loraine; De Santi, Susan & Erlich, Jonathan (eds.), Discourse Analysis and Applications: Studies in Adult Clinical Populations. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, John. (1984) “A Change-of-State Token and Aspects of Its Sequential Placement. In Dwight Atkinson & John Heritage (eds.), Structures of Social Action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 299–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, John. (1998a) “Oh-prefaced responses to inquiry.” Language in Society 27: 291–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, John. (1998b) “Oh-Prefacing: A Method of Modifying Agreement/Disagreement.” In Cecilia Ford et al.(eds.), The Language of Turn and Sequence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, Alison. (2003) “So…?: Pragmatic Implications of So-Prefaced Questions in Formal Police Interviews.” In Janet Cotterill (ed.), Language in the Legal Process. Basingstoke and NY: Palgrave Macmillan, 91–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jucker, Andreas. (1993) “The discourse marker well: A relevance-theoretical account.” Journal of Pragmatics 19: 435–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jucker, Andreas, Sara Smith & Tanja Luedge. (2003) “Interactive aspects of vagueness in conversation.” Journal of Pragmatics 35: 1737–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasher, Asa (ed.). (1998) Pragmatics: Critical Concepts, Vol. VI: Pragmatics, Grammar, Psychology; Sociology. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katayama, H. (2001) “Beyond ‘change-of-state’: ‘oh’ as a facilitator of teacher-student interactions in an ESL conversation class.” Crossroads of Language, Interaction, and Culture 4 [n.p.]. Retrieved originally from 2001 Conference Abstracts, http://www.sscnet.ucla.edu/clic/

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzenberger, I. (2004) “The development of clause packaging in spoken and written texts.” Journal of Pragmatics 36: 1921–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, Joerg & Trixi Rech (1998) “Towards a modular description of the deficits in spontaneous speech in dementia.” Journal of Pragmatics 29: 313–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempler, D. (1995) “Language Changes in Dementia of the Alzheimer Type.” In R. Lubinsky (ed.), Dementia and Communication: Research and Clinical Implications. San Diego: Singular, pp. 98–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Jung Hee, Michael Glass, Reva Freedman & Martha Evens. (2000) “Learning the use of discourse markers in tutorial dialogue for an intelligent tutoring system.” Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, CogSci, 2000, Philadelphia, 262–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linguistics Data Consortium. (2004) “Annotation guidelines: metadata (as resumptive of turn),” last accessed 25 June 2004 at http://www.ldc.upenn.edu/Projects/MDE/

    Google Scholar 

  • Macaulay, Ron. (2002) “You know, it depends.” Journal of Pragmatics 34: 749–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, William C. & Sandra A. Thompson. (1988) “Rhetorical Structure Theory: towards a functional theory of text organization.” Text, 8(3): 243–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahendra, N., Bayles, K.A. & Tomoeda, C.K. (1999) “Effect of an unfamiliar accent on the repetition ability of normal elders and individuals with Alzheimer’s disease.” Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 7: 223–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, N. (2003) “Intelligibility and negotiated meaning in interaction.” Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 17: 317–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norrick, Neal. (2000) Conversational Narrative: Storytelling in Everyday Talk. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Obler, Loraine & De Santi, Susan. (2000) “Eliciting Language from Patients with Alzheimer’s Disease.” Pages 403–16 in Lise Menn & Nan Ratner (eds.), Methods for Studying Language Production. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, L., A. Whitworth & R. Lesser. (1998) “Conversing in dementia: a conversation analytic approach.” Journal of Neurolinguistics 11: 33–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, Lisa, Anne Whitworth & Ruth Lesser. (1997) Conversation Analysis Profile for People with Cognitive Impairment. London: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, Michael. (1998) “Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders.” Journal of Pragmatics 29 (1998): 291–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramanathan, Vai. (1997) Alzheimer Discourse: Some Sociolinguistic Dimensions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhys, Catrin & Nicola Schmidt-Renfree. (2000) “Facework, social politeness and the Alzheimer’s patient.” Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 14: 533–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ripich, D. & B. Terrell. (1988) “Patterns of discourse cohesion and coherence in Alzheimer’s disease.” Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 53: 8–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ripich, D.N., Carpenter, B. & Ziol, E. (2000) “Conversational cohesion in men and women with Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal study.” International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35(1): 49–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, Sheldon & Leonard Abbeduto. (1987) “Indicators of linguistic competence in the peer group conversational behavior of mildly retarded adults.” Applied Psycholinguistics 8: 19–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, Ellen, S. Meredith, M. MacLean & J. B. Orange. (1995) “Changing the way we talk with elders: promoting health using the Communication Enhancement Model.” International Journal of Aging and Human Development 41: 89–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendle-Short, Johanna. (2003) “So what does this show us?”: analysis of the discourse marker ‘so’ in seminar talk.” Australian Review of Applied Linguistics 26: 46–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabat, S.R. & Collins, M. (1999) “Intact social, cognitive ability, and selfood: A case-study of Alzheimer’s disease.” American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 14: 11–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Safarova Marie, Swerts Marc. (2004) “On recognition of declarative questions in English.” Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2004 Conference, Nara (Japan), March 23–26, 2004, pp. 313–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffrin, Deborah. (1987) Discourse Markers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schourup, Lawrence. (2001) “Rethinking well.” Journal of Pragmatics 33: 1025–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schrock, J.C. & Fox Tree, J.E. (2000) “‘So’ and ‘and’ in spontaneous speech.” Poster presented at the 2000 meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA. Last accessed 15 June 2004 at http://psychology.gatech.edu/renglelab/chadpage/So_and_And.ppt

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Small, J.A., G. Gutman, S. Makela & B. Hillhouse. (2003) “Strategies used by caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease during activities of daily living.” Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46: 353–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stemmer, Brigitte & Paul Schoenle. (2000) “Neuropragmatics in the 21st Century.” Brain and Language 71 (special issue).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tappen, R.M., C. Williams-Burgess, J. Edelstein, T. Touhy & S. Fishman. (1997) “Communicating with individuals with Alzheimer’s disease: an examination of recommended strategies.” Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 11(5): 249–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, Valerie; Sabat, Steven; Koger, Rolf. (1999) “Intact use of politeness in the discourse of Alzheimer’s sufferers.” Language and Communication 19: 163–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Craen, Piet. (2000) “Non-standard Dutch ‘Allez’ as a discourse particle.” Conference on Discourse particles, modal and focal particles, and all that stuff, Universitaire Stichting, Brussels, December 2000. Abstract: http://odur.let.rug.nl/~vdwouden/particles/prog02.htm

    Google Scholar 

  • Waring, Hansun Zhang. (2002) “Displaying substantive recipiency in seminar discussion.” Research on Language and Social Interaction, 2002, Vol. 35: 453–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wray, Alison. (2002) Formulaic Language and the Lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wray, A. & Perkins, M. (2000) “The functions of formulaic language: an integrated model.” Language and Communication 20(1): 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2005 Boyd H. Davis

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Davis, B.H. (2005). So, You had Two Sisters, Right? Functions for Discourse Markers in Alzheimer’s Talk. In: Davis, B.H. (eds) Alzheimer Talk, Text and Context. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230502024_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics